Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 19, Issue 1, pp 239–243 | Cite as

Evidence for the perceptual origin of right-sided feeding biases in cetaceans

  • Karina Karenina
  • Andrey Giljov
  • Tatiana Ivkovich
  • Yegor MalashichevEmail author
Short Communication

Abstract

Foraging behaviour of many cetacean species features the side biases at the population level. The origin of these behavioural lateralisations remains generally unclear. Here we explored lateralisation in aerial display of resident orcas in different behavioural contexts. Side preferences were analysed in lunging during foraging and breaching. One event of each type of displays per individually identified orca was used for analysis. Orcas showed a population-level preference to lunge on the right side when foraging (75 % of lunges). In contrast, no lateralisation was found in breaching (54 % of breaches to the right, 45 % to the left). The right-sided bias in foraging found in orcas is in line with evidence from other whales, both baleen and toothed, and confirms the uniformity of feeding biases among cetaceans. In contrast to breaching, lunging in orcas was associated with fish pursuit, that is, with focused attention to and sensory perception of prey stimulus. The emergence of lateralisation in lunging and the absence of significant bias in breaching suggest that feeding biases in whales are underpinned by sensory lateralisation, that is, by lateralised hemispheric processing of sensory information about the prey. Evidence from orcas may be extrapolated to other cetaceans since right-sided biases in lunging during foraging is a very widespread phenomenon and likely to have a common origin. Our findings support the hypothesis that right-sided feeding biases are determined by left-hemisphere specialisation.

Keywords

Lateralisation Left hemisphere Cetaceans Breaching Lunge feeding Laterality in whales 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Erich Hoyt, Alexandr Burdin, Mikhail Nagaylik, Mikhail Guzeev, Ekaterina Borisova and Anton Biatov for their help in the field work organisation and data collection. We would like to acknowledge Janeane Ingram for language corrections. Expeditions were supported by Whale and Dolphin Conservation (WDC), the Rufford Maurice Laing Foundation, the Humane Society International, and Animal Welfare Institute. Data processing and analytical work were supported by the Russian Scientific Fund (Grant #14-14-00284).

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

Data collection was carried out as part of a complex population monitoring programme aimed at collecting general data on the behaviour, distribution, and ecology of orcas. Every effort was made to minimise possible disturbance to the orcas. Approach and observation of cetaceans for research purposes requires no special permissions according to local laws and regulations in Russia. Usual whale-watching guidelines applied in USA to minimise the possible negative impact on whales were followed. This article does not contain any studies involving human participants performed by any of the authors.

Supplementary material

Online Resource 1

(Video) Orca lunging on the right side during foraging. (MPG 2146 kb)

10071_2015_899_MOESM2_ESM.mpg (6.6 mb)
Online Resource 2 (Video) Orca breaching on the right side. (MPG 6782 kb)
10071_2015_899_MOESM3_ESM.pdf (16 kb)
Online Resource 3 (Table) Orcas individual lunging and breaching events in the same individuals (PDF 15 kb)

References

  1. Canning C, Crain D, Eaton TS, Nuessly K, Friedlaender A, Hurst T, Parks S, Ware C, Wiley D, Weinrich M (2011) Population-level lateralized feeding behaviour in North Atlantic humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae. Anim Behav 82(4):901–909. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.0 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Clapham PJ, Leimkuhler E, Gray BK, Mattila DK (1995) Do humpback whales exhibit lateralized behaviour? Anim Behav 50(1):73–82. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1995.0222 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Fox AG (2010) Interactions between birds and strand-feeding dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in a South Carolina salt marsh. Doctoral dissertation, Coastal Carolina UniversityGoogle Scholar
  4. Geise L, Gomes N, Cerqueira R (1999) Behaviour, habitat use and population size of Sotalia fluviatilis (Gervais, 1853) (Cetacea, Delphinidae) in the Cananéia estuary region, São Paulo, Brazil. Rev Bras Biol 59(2):183–194. doi: 10.1590/S0034-71081999000200002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Giljov AN, Karenina KA, Malashichev YB (2009) An eye for a worm: lateralisation of feeding behaviour in aquatic anamniotes. Laterality 14(3):273–286. doi: 10.1080/13576500802379665 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Ivkovich T, Filatova OA, Burdin AM, Sato H, Hoyt E (2010) The social organization of resident-type killer whales (Orcinus orca) in Avacha Gulf, Northwest Pacific, as revealed through association patterns and acoustic similarity. Z Saugetierkd 75:198–210. doi: 10.1016/j.mambio.2009.03.006 Google Scholar
  7. Karenina K, Giljov A, Glazov D, Malashichev Y (2013a) Social laterality in wild beluga whale infants: comparisons between locations, escort conditions, and ages. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67(7):1195–1204. doi: 10.1007/s00265-013-1545-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Karenina K, Giljov A, Ivkovich T, Burdin A, Malashichev Y (2013b) Lateralization of spatial relationships between wild mother and infant orcas, Orcinus orca. Anim Behav 86(6):1225–1231. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Lewis JS, Schroeder WW (2003) Mud plume feeding, a unique foraging behavior of the bottlenose dolphin in the Florida Keys. Gulf Mexico Sci 21:92–97Google Scholar
  10. Liebal K, Call J (2012) The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 367:118–128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0044 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Lippolis G, Joss JMP, Rogers LJ (2009) Australian lungfish (Neoceratodus forsteri): a missing link in the evolution of complementary side biases for predator avoidance and prey capture. Brain Behav Evol 73:295–303. doi: 10.1159/000230674 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. MacNeilage PF (2013) Vertebrate whole-body-action asymmetries and the evolution of right handedness: a comparison between humans and marine mammals. Dev Psychobiol 55(6):577–587. doi: 10.1002/dev.21114 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Marino L (2006) Evolution of the brain and cognition in cetaceans. In: Platek SM, Keenan JP, Shackelford TK (eds) Evolutionary cognitive neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 163–193Google Scholar
  14. Noren DP, Johnson AH, Rehder D, Larson A (2009) Close approaches by vessels elicit surface active behaviors by southern resident killer whales. Endanger Species Res 8(3):179–192. doi: 10.3354/esr00205 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Pyenson ND, Goldbogen JA, Vogl AW, Szathmary G, Drake RL, Shadwick RE (2012) Discovery of a sensory organ that coordinates lunge feeding in rorqual whales. Nature 485:498–501. doi: 10.1038/nature11135 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G, Andrew RJ (2013) Divided brains: the biology and behavior of brain asymmetries. Cambridge University Press, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Sakai M, Hishii T, Takeda S, Kohshima S (2006) Laterality of flipper rubbing behaviour in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus): caused by asymmetry of eye use? Behav Brain Res 170(2):204–210. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.02.018 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Saulitis E, Matkin C, Barrett-Lennard L, Heise K, Ellis G (2000) Foraging strategies of sympatric killer whale (Orcinus orca) populations in Prince William Sound, Alaska. Mar Mamm Sci 16(1):94–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2000.tb00906.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Silber GK, Fertl D (1995) Intentional beaching by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Colorado River Delta, Mexico. Aquat Mamm 21:183–186Google Scholar
  20. Siniscalchi M, Dimatteo S, Pepe AM, Sasso R, Quaranta A (2012) Visual lateralization in wild striped dolphins (Stenella coeruleoalba) in response to stimuli with different degrees of familiarity. PLoS One 7(1):e30001. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030001 PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Tershy BR, Wiley DN (1992) Asymmetrical pigmentation in the fin whale: a test of two feeding related hypotheses. Mar Mamm Sci 8(3):315–318. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.1992.tb00416.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Vallortigara G, Chiandetti C, Sovrano VA (2011) Brain asymmetry (animal). WIREs Cogn Sci 2:146–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Vaughn RL, Degrati M, McFadden CJ (2010) Dusky dolphins foraging in daylight. In: Wursig B, Wursig M (eds) The dusky dolphin: master acrobat off different shores. Academic Press, New York, pp 115–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ventolini N, Ferrero EA, Sponza S, Della Chiesa A, Zucca P, Vallortigara G (2005) Laterality in the wild: preferential hemifield use during predatory and sexual behaviour in the black-winged stilt. Anim Behav 69:1077–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.09.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Volkova EV, Ivkovich TV, Shitova MV, Cherniaeva EN, Nagaylik MM, Burdin AM (2014) Prey preferences of fish-eating killer whales of Avacha Gulf during the summer period. In: Materials of III scientific conference “Behavior and behavioral ecology of mammals”. Chernogolovka, 14–18 April, p 26Google Scholar
  26. Woodward BL, Winn JP (2006) Apparent lateralized behavior in gray whales feeding off the central British Columbia coast. Mar Mamm Sci 22(1):64–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-7692.2006.00006.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Karina Karenina
    • 1
  • Andrey Giljov
    • 1
  • Tatiana Ivkovich
    • 1
  • Yegor Malashichev
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  1. 1.Department of Vertebrate Zoology, Faculty of BiologySaint Petersburg State UniversitySaint PetersburgRussia
  2. 2.Laboratory of Neuroecology, Department of Ecological PhysiologyInstitute of Experimental MedicineSaint PetersburgRussia

Personalised recommendations