Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 18, Issue 1, pp 111–118 | Cite as

Testing problem solving in turkey vultures (Cathartes aura) using the string-pulling test

  • Anne Margaret EllisonEmail author
  • Jane Watson
  • Eric Demers
Original Paper

Abstract

To examine problem solving in turkey vultures (Cathartes aura), six captive vultures were presented with a string-pulling task, which involved drawing a string up to access food. This test has been used to assess cognition in many bird species. A small piece of meat suspended by a string was attached to a perch. Two birds solved the problem without apparent trial-and-error learning; a third bird solved the problem after observing a successful bird, suggesting that this individual learned from the other vulture. The remaining birds failed to complete the task. The successful birds significantly reduced the time needed to solve the task from early trials compared to late trials, suggesting that they had learned to solve the problem and improved their technique. The successful vultures solved the problem in a novel way: they pulled the string through their beak with their tongue, and may have gathered the string in their crop until the food was in reach. In contrast, ravens, parrots and finches use a stepwise process; they pull the string up, tuck it under foot, and reach down to pull up another length. As scavengers, turkey vultures use their beak for tearing and ripping at carcasses, but possess large, flat, webbed feet that are ill-suited to pulling or grasping. The ability to solve this problem and the novel approach used by the turkey vultures in this study may be a result of the unique evolutionary pressures imposed on this scavenging species.

Keywords

Turkey vulture Cathartes aura Cognition String-pulling 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Dr. Elliott Marchant for acting as a consultant during this project. We would also like to thank Gill and Robyn Radcliffe of Pacific Northwest Raptors, Julie Mackey of the North Island Wildlife Rescue and Devin Manky of the Grouse Mountain Wildlife Refuge for allowing us to work with their vultures, and for their continued support and enthusiasm. We would also like to thank Ron Ydenberg for funding the research at Grouse Mountain for his helpful insights and discussion. This study was funded by the Biology Department at Vancouver Island University.

Supplementary material

Online Resource 1 A turkey vulture (Judge Dredd) using a novel approach to solve the string-pulling problem (MPG 8494 kb)

References

  1. Afework B, Kamata N, Mioko K, Sugita S (2011) Quantity discrimination in jungle crows, Corvus macrorhynchos. Anim Behav 82:635–641. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.025 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Balda RP, Kamil AC, Bednekoff PA (1996) Predicting cognitive capacity from natural history: examples from four species of corvids. Curr Ornithol 13:33–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Biondi LM, García GO, Bó MS, Vassallo AI (2010) Social learning in the Caracara Chimango, Milvago chimango (Aves: Falconiformes): an age comparison. Ethology 116:722–735. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01794.x Google Scholar
  4. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1999) Scrub Jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) remember the relative time of caching as well as the location and content of their caches. J Comp Psychol 113:403–416PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2005) Corvid cognition. Curr Biol 15:80–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Clayton NS, Emery NJ (2007) The social life of corvids. Curr Biol 17:652–656CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Colbert-White EN, Monteen E, Sharpe DI (2013) String-pulling behaviour in a Harris’s Hawk Parabuteo unicinctus. Ibis 155:611–615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Devault TL, Rhodes OE, Shivik JA (2003) Scavenging by vertebrates: behavioral, ecological, and evolutionary perspectives on an important energy transfer pathway in terrestrial ecosystems. Oikos 2:225–234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2004) The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306:1903–1907. doi: 10.1126/science.1098410 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Emery NJ, Clayton NS, Western I (2002) Evolution of the avian brain and intelligence. Curr Biol 15:946–950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Evans BA, Sordahl TA (2009) Factors influencing perch selection by communally roosting turkey vultures. J Field Ornithol 80:364–372. doi: 10.1111/j.1557-9263.2009.00242.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Heinrich B (1995) An experimental investigation of insight in common ravens (Corvus corax). Auk 112:994–1003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Heinrich B (1999) Mind of the raven: Investigations and adventures with wolf-birds. Harper Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  14. Heinrich B, Bugnyar T (2005) Testing problem solving in ravens: string-pulling to reach food. Ethology 111:962–976CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hertel F (1994) Diversity in body size and feeding morphology within past and present vulture assemblages. Ecol 75:1074–1084CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hintze J (2007) NCSS 2007. NCSS, LLC., Kaysville, Utah. www.ncss.com
  17. Houston DC (1994) To the vultures belong the spoils. Nat Hist 103:34–36Google Scholar
  18. Kirk DA, Houston DC (1995) Social dominance in migrant and resident turkey vultures at carcasses: evidence for a despotic distribution? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 36:323–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Krasheninnikova A, Wanker R (2010) String-pulling in spectacled parrotlets (Forpus conspicillatus). Behaviour 147:725–739. doi: 10.1163/000579510X491072 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. McLean AN (2001) Cognitive abilities—the result of selective pressures on food acquisition? Appl Anim Behav Sci 71:241–258PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McVey KJ, Skrade PD, Sordahl TA (2008) Use of a communal roost by turkey vultures in Northeastern Iowa. J F Ornithol 79:170–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Overington SE, Dubois F, Lefebvre L (2008) Food unpredictability drives both generalism and social foraging: a game theoretical model. Behav Ecol 19:836–841. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arn037 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Palmer R (1988) Handbook of North American birds, vol 4. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  24. Pepperberg IM (1987) Evidence for conceptual quantitative abilities in the African grey parrot: labeling of cardinal sets. Ethology 75:37–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1987.tb00641.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pepperberg IM (2013) Abstract concepts: data from a grey parrot. Behav Process 93:82–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Platt SG, Rainwater TR (2009) Noteworthy observations of foraging turkey vultures. Wilson J Ornithol 121:839–841CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Reader SM, Laland KN (2002) Innovation and social learning: individual variation and brain evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:4436–4441PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Roth TC, LaDage LD, Pravosudov VV (2010) Learning capabilities enhanced in harsh environments: a common garden approach. Proc Biol Sci 277:3187–3193. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0630 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Sazima I (2007) From carrion-eaters to bathers’ bags plunderers: how black vultures (Coragyps atratus) could have found that plastic bags may contain food. Rev Bras Ornitol 15:617–620Google Scholar
  30. Seed A, Boogert N (2013) Animal cognition: an end to insight? Curr Biol 23:67–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Seibt U, Wickler W (2006) Individuality in problem solving: string pulling in two carduelis species (Aves: Passeriformes). Ethology 112:493–502. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01172.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Sekercioglu CH (2006) Increasing awareness of avian ecological function. Trends Ecol Evol 21:464–471. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2006.05.007 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Shettleworth SJ (1984) Learning and behavioral ecology. In: Davies NB, Krebs JR (eds) Behavioral ecology, 2nd edn. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, pp 170–194Google Scholar
  34. Slack KE, Delsuc F, Mclenachana PA, Arnasonb U, Penny D (2007) Resolving the root of the avian mitogenomic tree by breaking up long branches. Mol Phylogenet Evol 42:1–13PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Sol D, Timmermans S, Lefebvre L (2002) Behavioural flexibility and invasion success in birds. Anim Behav 63:495–502. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1953 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Stoyanova Y, Stefanov N, Schmutz JK (2010) Twig used as a tool by the Egyptian vulture (Neophron percnopterus). J Raptor Res 44:154–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Taylor AH, Hunt GR, Medina FS, Gray RD (2009) Do New Caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning? Proc R Soc B 276:247–254. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.1107 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Taylor AH, Medina FS, Holzhaider JC, Hearne LJ, Hunt GR, Gray RD (2010) An investigation into the cognition behind spontaneous string pulling in New Caledonian crows. PLoS One 5:e9345. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009345 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Tebbich S, Seed AM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2007) Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem. Anim Cogn 10:225–231. doi: 10.1007/s10071-006-0061-4 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Turner AM, Turner SE, Lappi HM (2006) Learning, memory and predator avoidance by freshwater snails: effects of experience on predator recognition and defensive strategy. Anim Behav 72:1443–1450. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.05.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Van Horik J, Emery NJ (2011) Evolution of cognition. Cogn Sci 2:621–633. doi: 10.1002/wcs.144 Google Scholar
  42. van Lawick-Goodall J, van Lawick-Goodall H (1966) Use of tools by the Egyptian vulture, Neophron percnopterus. Nature 212:1468–1469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Wasserman EA, Nagasaka Y, Castro L, Brzykcy SJ (2013) Pigeons learn virtual patterned-string problems in a computerized touch screen environment. Anim Cogn 16:737–753. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0608-0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Werdenich D, Huber L (2006) A case of quick problem solving in birds: string pulling in keas (Nestor notabilis). Anim Behav 71:855–863CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Wink M (1995) Phylogeny of old and new world vultures (Aves: Accipitridae and Cathartidae) inferred from nucleotide sequences of the mitochondrial cytochrome b gene. J Biosci 11:868–871Google Scholar
  46. Witoslawski JJ, Hanson HM (1963) Behavioural studies with a black vulture Coragyps atratus. J Exp Anal Behav 6:605–606PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zar JH (2010) Biostatistical analysis, 5th edn. Pearson Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle RiverGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anne Margaret Ellison
    • 1
    • 2
    Email author
  • Jane Watson
    • 2
  • Eric Demers
    • 2
  1. 1.Simon Fraser UniversityBurnabyCanada
  2. 2.Vancouver Island UniversityNanaimoCanada

Personalised recommendations