Animal Cognition

, Volume 17, Issue 3, pp 619–631 | Cite as

Rapid cognitive flexibility of rhesus macaques performing psychophysical task-switching

  • Ema Avdagic
  • Greg Jensen
  • Drew Altschul
  • Herbert S. Terrace
Original Paper

Abstract

Three rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) performed a simultaneous chaining task in which stimuli had to be sorted according to their visual properties. Each stimulus could vary independently along two dimensions (luminosity and radius), and a cue indicating which dimension to sort by was random trial to trial. These rapid and unpredictable changes constitute a task-switching paradigm, in which subjects must encode task demands and shift to whichever task-set is presently activated. In contrast to the widely reported task-switching delay observed in human studies, our subjects show no appreciable reduction in reaction times following a switch in the task requirements. Also, in contrast to the results of studies on human subjects, monkeys experienced enduring interference from trial-irrelevant stimulus features, even after exhaustive training. These results are consistent with a small but growing body of evidence that task-switching in rhesus macaques differs in basic ways from the pattern of behavior reported in studies of human cognition. Given the importance of task-switching paradigms in cognitive and clinical assessment, and the frequency with which corresponding animal models rely on non-human primates, understanding these differences in behavior is essential to the comparative study of cognitive impairment.

Keywords

Comparative cognition Cognitive assessment Mental set Task-switching Rhesus macaques 

References

  1. Allport A, Styles EA, Hsieh S (1994) Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In: Ultimá C, Moscovitch M (eds) Attention and performance XV: conscious and non-conscious information processing. Bradford, Cambridge, MA, pp 421–452Google Scholar
  2. Behar I (1974) Performance of rhesus monkeys on two-cue problems with cues separated. Anim Learn Behav 2:49–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beran MJ, Pate JL, Washburn DA, Rumbaugh DM (2004) Sequential responding and planning in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 30:203–212PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berg EA (1948) A simple objective technique for measuring flexibility in thinking. J Gen Psychol 39:15–22PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Braver TS, Reynolds JR, Donaldson DI (2003) Neural mechanisms of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Neuron 39:713–726PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bryan J, Luszcz MA (2000) Measurement of executive function: considerations for detecting adult age differences. J Clinic Exp Neuropsychol 22:40–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Caselli L, Chelazzi L (2011) Does the macaque monkey provide a good model for studying human executive control? A comparative behavioral study of task switching. PLoS ONE 6:e21489PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clapp WC, Rubens MT, Sabharwal J, Gazzaley A (2011) Deficit in switching between functional brain networks underlies the impact of multitasking on working memory in older adults. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 108:7212–7217PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dixon P (1981) Algorithms and selective attention. Mem Cogn 9:177–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dreher JC, Grafman J (2003) Dissociating the roles of the rostral anterior cingulated and the lateral prefrontal cortices in performing two tasks simultaneously or successively. Cereb Cortex 13:329–339PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dreisbach G, Goschke T, Haider H (2007) The role of task rules and stimulus-response mappings in the task switching paradigm. Psychol Res 71:383–392PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dux PE, Ivanoff J, Asplund CL, Marois R (2006) Isolation of a central bottleneck of information processing with time-resolved fMRI. Neuron 52:1109–1120PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gómez JC (2005) Species comparative studies and cognitive development. Trends Cogn Sci 9:118–125PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gu BM, Park JY, Kang DH, Lee SJ, Yoo SY, Jo HJ, Choi CH, Lee JM, Kwon JS (2008) Neural correlates of cognitive flexibility during task-switching in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 131:155–164PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hentschke H, Stüttgen MC (2011) Computation of measures of effect size for neuroscience data sets. Eur J Neurosci 34:1887–1894PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Jensen G, Altschul D, Danly E, Terrace HS (2013a) Transfer of a serial representation between two distinct tasks by Rhesus macaques. PLoS ONE 8:e70285PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Jensen G, Ward RD, Balsam PD (2013b) Information: theory, brain, and behavior. J Exp Anal Behav 100:TBDGoogle Scholar
  18. Jersild AT (1927) Mental set and shift. Arch Psychol 89:1–82Google Scholar
  19. Kamigaki T, Fukushima T, Miyashita Y (2009) Cognitive set reconfiguration signals by macaque posterior parietal neurons. Neuron 61:941–951PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kleinsorge T, Heuer H (1999) Hierarchical switching in a multi-dimensional task space. Psychol Res 62:300–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Koch I (2001) Automatic and intentional activation of task sets. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 27:1474–1486PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kornell N, Son LK, Terrace HS (2007) Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychol Sci 18:64–71PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lashley KS (1951) The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Jeffress LA (ed) Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. Wiley, New York, pp 112–131Google Scholar
  24. Logan GD, Gordon RD (2001) Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. Psychol Rev 108:393–434PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. MacLeod CM (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull 109:163–203PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mantel N, Haenszel W (1959) Statistical aspects of the analysis of data from retrospective studies of disease. J Nat Cancer Inst 22:719–748PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Mayr U, Kliegl R (2000) Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 26:1124–1140PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mecklinger A, von Cramon DY, Springer A, Matthes-von Cramon G (1999) Executive control functions in task switching: evidence from brain injured patients. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 21:606–619PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Monsell S (2003) Task switching. Trends Cogn Neurosci 7:134–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Monsell S, Yeung N, Azuma R (2000) Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to switch to the weaker task? Psychol Res 63:250–264PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Moore TL, Killiany RJ, Rosene DL, Prusty S, Hollander W, Moss MB (2002) Impairment of executive function induced by hypertension in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Behav Neurosci 116:387–396PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Moore TL, Killiany RJ, Herndon JG, Rosene DL, Moss MB (2003) Impairment in abstraction and set shifting in aged rhesus monkeys. Neurobiol Aging 24:125–134PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Moore TL, Killiany RJ, Herndon JG, Rosene DL, Moss MB (2005) A non-human primate test of abstraction and set shifting: an automated adaptation of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. J Neurosci Method 146:165–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Myers JL, Well AD (2003) Research design and statistical analysis, 2nd edn. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJGoogle Scholar
  35. Nakahara K, Hayashi T, Konishi S, Miyashita Y (2002) Functional MRI of macaque monkeys performing a cognitive set-shifting task. Science 295:1532–1536PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ophir E, Nass C, Wagner AD (2009) Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 106:15583–15587PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pashler H (1994) Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. Psychol Bull 116:220–244PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pashler H (2000) Task switching and multitask performance. In: Monsell S, Driver J (eds) Attention and performance XVIII: control of cognitive processes. Bradford, Cambridge, MA, pp 277–307Google Scholar
  39. Prado J, Mutreja R, Booth JR (2013) Fractionating the neural substrates of transitive reasoning: task-dependent contributions of spatial and verbal representations. Cereb Cortex 23:499–507PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rogers RD, Monsell S (1995) Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. J Exp Psychol Gen 124:207–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Rossi A, Daneluzzo E, Mattei P, Bustini M, Casacchia M, Stratta P (1997) Wisconsin card sorting test and Stroop test performances in Schizophrenia: a shared construct. Neurosci Lett 226:87–90PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sakagami M, Pan X, Uttl B (2006) Behavioral inhibition and prefrontal cortex in decision-making. Neural Netw 19:1255–1265PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Scarf D, Danly E, Morgan G, Colombo M, Terrace HS (2011) Sequential planning in rhesus monkeys. Anim Cogn 14:317–324PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Spector A, Biederman I (1976) Mental set and mental shift revisited. Am J Psychol 89:669–679CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Stoet G, Snyder LH (2003a) Executive control and task-switching in monkeys. Neuropsychologia 41:1357–1364PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Stoet G, Snyder LH (2003b) Task preparation in macaque monkeys. Anim Cogn 6:121–130PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Stoet G, Snyder LH (2007) Extensive practice does not eliminate human switch costs. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 7:192–197PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Stoewer S, Ku SP, Goense J, Stdeudel T, Logothetis NK, Duncan J, Sigala N (2010) Frontoparietal activity with minimal decision and control in awake macaque at 7 T. Magn Reson Imaging 28:1120–1128PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Subiaul F, Cantlon JF, Holloway RL, Terrace HS (2004) Cognitive imitation in rhesus macaques. Science 305:407–410PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sudevan P, Taylor DA (1987) The cuing and priming of cognitive operations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 13:89–103PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Terrace HS (1984) Simultaneous chaining: the problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In: Commons ML, Herrnstein RJ, Wagner AR (eds) Quantitative analyses of behavior: discrimination processes. Ballinger, Cambridge, MA, pp 115–138Google Scholar
  52. Terrace HS (2005) The simultaneous chain: a new approach to serial learning. Trends Cogn Sci 9:202–210PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Terrace HS, Son LK, Brannon EM (2003) Serial expertise of rhesus macaques. Psychol Sci 14:66–73PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Washburn DA (1994) Stroop-like effects for monkeys and humans: processing speed or strength of association? Psychol Sci 5:375–379PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Weed MR, Bryant R, Perry S (2008) Cognitive development in macaques: attentional set-shifting in juvenile and adult rhesus monkeys. Neuroscience 157:22–28PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Whelan D (2008) Effective analysis of reaction time data. Psychol Rec 58:475–482Google Scholar
  57. Zeamer A, Decamp E, Clark K, Schneider JS (2011) Attention, executive functioning and memory in normal aged rhesus monkeys. Behav Brain Res 219:23–30PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ema Avdagic
    • 1
  • Greg Jensen
    • 1
  • Drew Altschul
    • 2
  • Herbert S. Terrace
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyColumbia UniversityNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of EdinburghEdinburghScotland, UK

Personalised recommendations