Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 17, Issue 2, pp 349–357 | Cite as

Vocal group signatures in the goitred gazelle Gazella subgutturosa

  • Ilya A. Volodin
  • Elena V. Volodina
  • Ekaterina N. Lapshina
  • Kseniya O. Efremova
  • Natalia V. Soldatova
Original Paper

Abstract

The potential for vocal modification in mammals has recently been of great interest. This study focuses on the potential for vocal matching in juvenile and adolescent goitred gazelles Gazella subgutturosa that were group housed as part of an animal management programme. Two groups of animals (16 and 19 unrelated individuals, respectively) were recorded at two different ages, juvenile and adolescent, regarding 20–25 calls per individual per age; each group was evaluated in a separate year. Vocal similarity of group members compared to non-members was prominent in both ages, but higher in juveniles. Individual identity was prominent in both ages and higher in adolescents. The more prominent vocal indicators of group membership in juveniles could be related to their higher social dependence compared to adolescents. The more individualized calls of adolescents could be a mechanistic consequence of more stable growth at older age. Our results suggest vocal plasticity of goitred gazelles under social influences. These data add to recent evidence about domestic goat kids Capra hircus, suggesting that vocalizations of species that are not capable of imitation are more flexible than previously thought.

Keywords

Mammal Ungulate Vocal communication Vocal development Social effects Production vocal learning 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff of the Djeiran Ecocenter for help and support and V. Matrosova for assistance with data collection. We thank R. Frey for help with the anatomical background for establishing formant settings and O. Sibiryakova for help with analysis of literature. We thank V. Ivanitsky, I. Marova and especially O. Filatova for help with statistics. We thank two anonymous reviewers for their valuable and encouraging comments. The study was supported by the Russian Foundation for Basic Research, Grant 12-04-00260a (for IV, EV, EL and KE).

References

  1. Arriaga G, Jarvis ED (2013) Mouse vocal communication system: are ultrasounds learned or innate? Brain Lang 124:96–116PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arriaga G, Zhou EP, Jarvis ED (2012) Of mice, birds, and men: the mouse ultrasonic song system has some features similar to humans and song-learning birds. PLoS One 7(10):e46610. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046610 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Blank DA (1998) Mating behavior of the Persian gazelle Gazella subgutturosa Güldenstaedt, 1780. Mammalia 62:499–519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blank DA, Ruckstuhl K, Yang W (2012) Influence of population density on group sizes in goitered gazelle (Gazella subgutturosa Guld., 1780). Eur J Wild Res 58:981–989CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Boersma P, Weenink D (2009) Praat: doing phonetics by computer. http://www.praat.org/
  6. Boughman J (1997) Greater spear-nosed bats give group-distinctive calls. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 40:61–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boughman J (1998) Vocal learning by greater spear-nosed bats. Proc R Soc Lond B 265:227–233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Briefer EF (2012) Vocal expression of emotions in mammals: mechanisms of production and evidence. J Zool 288:1–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Briefer EF, McElligott AG (2011a) Indicators of age, body size and sex in goat kid calls revealed using the source–filter theory. Appl Anim Behav Sci 133:175–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Briefer EF, McElligott AG (2011b) Mutual mother-offspring recognition in an ungulate hider species (Capra hircus). Anim Cogn 14:585–598PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Briefer EF, McElligott AG (2012) Social effects on vocal ontogeny in an ungulate, the goat, Capra hircus. Anim Behav 83:991–1000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Crockford C, Herbinger I, Vigiland L, Boesch C (2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: a case for vocal learning? Ethology 110:221–243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Efremova KO, Volodin IA, Volodina EV, Frey R, Lapshina EN, Soldatova NV (2011) Developmental changes of nasal and oral calls in the goitred gazelle Gazella subgutturosa, a nonhuman mammal with a sexually dimorphic and descended larynx. Naturwissenschaften 98:919–931PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fitch WT (2000) The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends Cogn Sci 4:258–267PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Fitch WT (2010) The evolution of language. Cambridge Univ Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fitch WT, Reby D (2001) The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1669–1675CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Frey R, Volodin IA, Volodina EV, Soldatova NV, Juldaschev ET (2011) Descended and mobile larynx, vocal tract elongation and rutting roars in male goitred gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa Güldenstaedt, 1780). J Anat 218:566–585PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Janik VM, Slater PJB (1997) Vocal learning in mammals. Adv Stud Behav 26:59–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Janik VM, Slater PJB (2000) The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Anim Behav 60:1–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jones G, Ransome RD (1993) Echolocation calls of bats are influenced by maternal effects and change over a lifetime. Proc R Soc Lond B 252:125–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kingswood SC, Blank DA (1996) Gazella subgutturosa. Mammalian species 518:1–10Google Scholar
  22. Knörnschild M, von Helversen O (2008) Nonmutual vocal mother-pup recognition in the greater sac-winged bat. Anim Behav 76:1001–1009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Knörnschild M, Nagy M, Metz M, Mayer F, von Helversen O (2010) Complex vocal imitation during ontogeny in a bat. Biol Lett 6:156–159PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Knörnschild M, Nagy M, Metz M, Mayer F, von Helversen O (2012) Learned vocal group signatures in the polygynous bat Saccopteryx bilineata. Anim Behav 84:761–769CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kruchenkova EP (2009) Maternal behaviour of mammals. Krasand, Moscow (in Russian)Google Scholar
  26. Lapshina EN, Volodin IA, Volodina EV, Frey R, Efremova KO, Soldatova NV (2012) The ontogeny of acoustic individuality in the nasal calls of captive goitred gazelles, Gazella subgutturosa. Behav Process 90:323–330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lemasson A, Gautier J-P, Hausberger M (2003) Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell’s monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli). C R Biol 326:1185–1193PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Petit EJ, Zuberbühler K (2011) Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? BMC Evol Biol 11:362. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-11-362 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lieblich AK, Symmes D, Newman JD, Shapiro M (1980) Development of the isolation peep in laboratory-bred squirrel monkeys. Anim Behav 28:1–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lingle S, Wyman MT, Kotrba R, Teichroeb LJ, Romanow CA (2012) What makes a cry a cry? A review of infant distress vocalizations. Curr Zool 58:698–726Google Scholar
  31. Packer C, Lewis S, Pusey A (1992) A comparative analysis of non-offspring nursing. Anim Behav 43:265–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Pereladova OB, Pereladov SV (1986) Reproduction of goitred gazelles in enclosures of Sunt-Hasargag Natural Reserve. In: Flint V (ed) Protection and perspectives of restoring population of goitred gazelles in USSR. VNIIPriroda, Moscow, pp 49–59Google Scholar
  33. Pereladova OB, Bahloul K, Sempere AJ, Soldatova NV, Schadilov UM, Prisiadznuk VE (1998) Influence of environmental factors on a population of goitred gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa subgutturosa Guldenstaedt, 1780) in semi-wild conditions in an arid environment: a preliminary study. J Arid Environ 39:577–591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Perla BS, Slobodchikoff CN (2002) Habitat structure and alarm call dialects in Gunnison’s prairie dog (Cynomys gunnisoni). Behav Ecol 13:844–850CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. R Development Core Team (2009) R foundation for statistical computing. R foundation for statistical computing, Vienna. http://www.R-project.org
  36. Reby D, Cargnelutti B, Hewison AJM (1999) Contexts and possible functions of barking in roe deer. Anim Behav 57:1121–1128PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rukstalis M, Fite JE, French JA (2003) Social change affects vocal structure in a callitrichid primate (Callitrix kuhlii). Ethology 109:327–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Sanvito S, Galimberti F, Miller EH (2007) Observational evidences of vocal learning in southern elephant seals: a longitudinal study. Ethology 113:137–146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schusterman RJ (2008) Vocal learning in mammals with special emphasis on pinnipeds. In: Oller DK, Gribel U (eds) The evolution of communicative flexibility: complexity, creativity, and adaptability in human and animal communication. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp 41–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (2010) Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Lang 115:92–100PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Snowdon CT, Elowson AM (1999) Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired. Ethology 105:893–908CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Soldatova NV (1983) To behaviour of goitred gazelles in Bukhara breeding centre. In: Flint V (ed) Ecology of plants and animals of natural reserves of Uzbekistan. Fan, Tashkent, pp 86–90 (in Russian)Google Scholar
  43. Soldatova N, Juldashev E, Volodin I, Volodina E, Efremova K, Lapshina E (2010) Keeping, raising and body mass dynamics of the goitred gazelle calves (Gazella subgutturosa) in captivity. Sci Res Zool Parks 26:64–71 (in Russian)Google Scholar
  44. Sorokin PA, Soldatova NV, Lukarevskiy VS, Kholodova MV (2011) Genetic diversity and relations of the goitered gazelle (Gazella subgutturosa) groups from Uzbekistan, Turkmenistan, and Azerbaijan: analysis of the D-loop of mitochondrial DNA. Biol Bull 38:585–590CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tanaka T, Sugiura H, Masataka N (2006) Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of the development of group differences in acoustic features of coo calls in two groups of Japanese macaques. Ethology 112:7–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Terrazas A, Serafin N, Hernandez H, Nowak R, Poindron P (2003) Early recognition of newborn goat kids by their mother: II. Auditory recognition and evidence of an individual acoustic signature in the neonate. Dev Psychobiol 43:311–320PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Townsend SW, Hollen LI, Manser MB (2010) Meerkat close calls encode group-specific signatures, but receivers fail to discriminate. Anim Behav 80:133–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tyack PL (1997) Development and social functions of signature whistles in bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus. Bioacoustics 8:21–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tyack PL (2008) Convergence of calls as animals form social bonds, active compensation for noisy communication channels, and the evolution of vocal learning in mammals. J Comp Psychol 122:319–331PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Volodin IA, Lapshina EN, Volodina EV, Frey R, Soldatova NV (2011) Nasal and oral calls in juvenile goitred gazelles (Gazella subgutturosa) and their potential to encode sex and identity. Ethology 117:294–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Weiß BM, Ladich F, Spong P, Symonds H (2006) Vocal behavior of resident killer whale matrilines with newborn calves: the role of family signatures. J Acoust Soc Am 119:627–635PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ilya A. Volodin
    • 1
  • Elena V. Volodina
    • 2
  • Ekaterina N. Lapshina
    • 1
  • Kseniya O. Efremova
    • 1
  • Natalia V. Soldatova
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Vertebrate Zoology, Faculty of BiologyLomonosov Moscow State UniversityMoscowRussia
  2. 2.Scientific Research DepartmentMoscowRussia
  3. 3.Ecocenter “Djeiran”BukharaRepublic of Uzbekistan

Personalised recommendations