Animal Cognition

, Volume 17, Issue 2, pp 317–336 | Cite as

The repertoire and intentionality of gestural communication in wild chimpanzees

  • Anna Ilona Roberts
  • Samuel George Bradley Roberts
  • Sarah-Jane Vick
Original Paper

Abstract

A growing body of evidence suggests that human language may have emerged primarily in the gestural rather than vocal domain, and that studying gestural communication in great apes is crucial to understanding language evolution. Although manual and bodily gestures are considered distinct at a neural level, there has been very limited consideration of potential differences at a behavioural level. In this study, we conducted naturalistic observations of adult wild East African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in order to establish a repertoire of gestures, and examine intentionality of gesture production, use and comprehension, comparing across manual and bodily gestures. At the population level, 120 distinct gesture types were identified, consisting of 65 manual gestures and 55 bodily gestures. Both bodily and manual gestures were used intentionally and effectively to attain specific goals, by signallers who were sensitive to recipient attention. However, manual gestures differed from bodily gestures in terms of communicative persistence, indicating a qualitatively different form of behavioural flexibility in achieving goals. Both repertoire size and frequency of manual gesturing were more affiliative than bodily gestures, while bodily gestures were more antagonistic. These results indicate that manual gestures may have played a significant role in the emergence of increased flexibility in great ape communication and social bonding.

Keywords

Gestural communication Gestural repertoire Intentionality Communicative persistence Chimpanzee Pan troglodytes 

Supplementary material

10071_2013_664_MOESM1_ESM.doc (35 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOC 35 kb)
10071_2013_664_MOESM2_ESM.doc (56 kb)
Supplementary material 2 (DOC 56 kb)
10071_2013_664_MOESM3_ESM.doc (154 kb)
Supplementary material 3 (DOC 154 kb)
10071_2013_664_MOESM4_ESM.zip (1666.8 mb)
Supplementary material 4 (ZIP 1.62 gb)

References

  1. Arbib MA, Liebal K, Pika S (2008) Primate vocalization, gesture, and the evolution of human language. Curr Anthropol 49:1052–1075. doi:10.1086/593015 Google Scholar
  2. Arcadi A, Robert D, Boesch C (1998) Buttress drumming by wild chimpanzees: temporal patterning, phrase integration into loud calls, and preliminary evidence for individual distinctiveness. Primates 39:505–518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Arcadi A, Robert D, Mugurusi F (2004) A comparison of buttress drumming by male chimpanzees from two populations. Primates 45:135–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bard KA (1992) Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Dev 63(5):1186–1197PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bruner J (1981) Intention in the structure of action and interaction. In: Lipsett L (ed) Advances in infancy research. Ablex, NorwoodGoogle Scholar
  6. Call J, Tomasello M (1996) The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In: Russon AE, Bard KA, Parker ST (eds) Reaching into thought: the minds of the great apes. Cambridge University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  7. Cartmill EA, Byrne RW (2010) Semantics of primate gestures: intentional meanings of orangutan gestures. Anim Cogn 13:793–804PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Corballis MC (2003) From mouth to hand: gesture, speech, and the evolution of right-handedness. Behav Brain Sci 26:199–208PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Corballis M (2009) Language as gesture. Hum Mov Sci 28:556–565PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Mundry R, Zuberbuehler K (2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Curr Biol 22:142–146PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Crockford C, Wittig R, Langergraber K, Ziegler T, Zuberbueler K, Deschner T (2013) Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proc Royal Soc B Biol Sci 280 (1755)Google Scholar
  12. Donald M (1991) Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  13. Dunbar RIM (1996) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  14. Forrester SG (2008) A multidimensional approach to investigations of behaviour: revealing structure in animal communication signals. Anim Behav 76(5):1749–1760CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Genty E, Breuer T, Hobaiter C, Byrne RW (2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Anim Cogn 12:527–546. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0213-4 PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Golinkoff RM (1986) I beg your pardon—the preverbal negotiation of failed messages. J Child Lang 13(3):455–476PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of gombe: patterns of behaviour. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  18. Hewes GW (1973) Primate communication and the gestural origin of language. Curr Anthropol 14:5–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hinde RA, Rowell TE (1962) Communication by posture and facial expression in the rhesus monkey. Proc Zool Soc Lond 138:1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hobaiter K, Byrne R (2011a) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Anim Cogn 14:745–767. doi:10.1007/s10071-011-0409-2 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hobaiter K, Byrne R (2011b) Serial gesturing by wild chimpanzees: its nature and function for communication. Anim Cogn 14:827–838. doi:10.1007/s10071-011-0416-3 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hopkins WD, Pika S, Liebal K, Bania A, Meguerditchian A, Gardner M, Schapiro SJ (2012) Handedness for manual gestures in great apes. In: Developments in Primate Gesture Research, vol 6, pp 93–111Google Scholar
  23. Krause MA, Fouts RS (1997) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) pointing: hand shapes, accuracy, and the role of eye gaze. Int J Comp Psychol 11:330–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD (1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees: a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Dev Psychol 34:813–822PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD, Bard KA (1996a) Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 110(4):346–353PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD, Bard KA (1996b) Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 110:346–353PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Leavens DA, Hostetter AB, Wesley MJ, Hopkins WD (2004) Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim Behav 67:467–476. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.04.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Leavens DA, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2005) Intentionality as measured in the persistence and elaboration of communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Child Dev 76:291–306PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Liebal K, Call J (2012) The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philos T R Soc B 367:118–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Liebal K, Call J, Tomasello M (2004) Use of gesture sequences in chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 64:377–396PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. McCarthy MS, Jensvold MLA, Fouts DH (2012) Use of gesture sequences in captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) play. Anim Cogn:1–11Google Scholar
  32. Meguerditchian A, Vauclair J, Hopkins WD (2010) Captive chimpanzees use their right hand to communicate with each other: implications for the origin of the cerebral substrate for language. Cortex 46:40–48PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mundry R, Fischer J (1998) Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often leads to incorrect P values: examples from animal behaviour. Anim Behav 56:256–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Nishida T, Koichiro Z, Takahisa M, Agumi I, McGrew WC (2010) Chimpanzee behavior in the wild: an audio-visual encyclopaedia. Springer, TokyoCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Parkinson B (1996) Emotions are social. Brit J Psychol 87:663–683PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Parr LA, Cohen M, Waal FD (2005) Influence of social context on the use of blended and graded facial displays in chimpanzees. Int J Primatol 26:73–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pika S, Mitani J (2006) Referential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Curr Biol 16:R191–R192PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pika S, Liebal K, Tomasello M (2005) Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): repertoire and use. Am J Primatol 65(1):39–61PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Plooij FX (1978) Some basic traits of language in wild chimpanzees. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. Academic press, London, pp 111–131Google Scholar
  40. Plooij FX (1979) How wild chimpanzee babies trigger the onset of mother-infant play–and what the mother makes of it. In: Bullowa M (ed) Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication. Cambridge University Press, New York, pp 223–243Google Scholar
  41. Plooij F (1984) Behavioural development of free-living chimpanzee babies and Infants. Ablex Publishing Corporation, NorwoodGoogle Scholar
  42. Pollick AS, de Waal FBM (2007) Ape gestures and language evolution. P Natl Acad Sci USA 104:8184–8189CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Puce A, Perrett D (2003) Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. Philos T R Soc B 358:435–445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Reynolds V (2005) The chimpanzees of the Budongo forest: ecology, behaviour, and conservation. Oxford University Press, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci 21:188–194PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Roberts AI, Vick S-J, Buchanan-Smith H (2012a) Usage and comprehension of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 84:459–470. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Roberts AI, Vick S-J, Roberts SGB, Buchanan-Smith HM, Zuberbühler K (2012b) A structure-based repertoire of manual gestures in wild chimpanzees: statistical analyses of a graded communication system. Evol Hum Behav 33(5):578–589. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.05.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Roberts AI, Vick S-J, Buchanan-Smith H (2013) Communicative intentions in wild chimpanzees: persistence and elaboration in gestural signalling. Anim Cogn 16(2):187–196. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0563-1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Scott N (2013) Gesture use by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): differences between sexes in inter- and intra-sexual interactions. Am J Primatol 75:555–567PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Seyfarth R, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Bergman T, Fischer J, Zuberbuhler K, Hammerschmidt K (2010) The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Anim Behav 80:3–8. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.04.012 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Slocombe KE, Waller BM, Liebal K (2011) The language void: the need for multimodality in primate communication research. Anim Behav 81:919–924. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.02.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Smith LW, Delgado R (2013) Considering the role of social dynamics and positional behaviour in gestural communication research. Am J PrimatolGoogle Scholar
  54. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  55. Tomasello M, Carpenter M (2007) Shared intentionality. Dev Sci 10(1):121–125. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Tomasello M, Frost GT (1989) A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates 30(1):35–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Tomasello M, Zuberbühler K (2002) Primate vocal and gestural communication. In: Bekoff M, Allen CS, Burghardt G (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  58. Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1984) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 37:137–154Google Scholar
  59. Tomasello M, George BL, Kruger AC, Jeffrey M, Evans FA (1985) The development of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. J Hum Evol 14:175–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1994) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 35(2):137–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tomasello M, Call J, Warren J, Frost T, Carpenter M, Nagell K (1997) The ontogeny of chimpanzee gestural signals: a comparison across groups and generations. Evol Com 1(2):223–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. van Hooff JARAM (1971) Aspects of the social behaviour and communication in human and higher non-human primates. Bronder-Offset, RotterdamGoogle Scholar
  63. van Lawick-Goodall J (1967) Mother-offspring relationships in free-ranging chimpanzees. In: Morris D (ed) Primate ethology. Weidenfeld and Nicolson, London, pp 287–346Google Scholar
  64. van Lawick-Goodall J (1968) A preliminary report on expressive movements and communication in the gombe stream chimpanzees. In: Jay P (ed) Primates: studies in adaptation and variability. Holt, Reinhart and Winston, New York, pp 313–374Google Scholar
  65. Wallez C, Schaeffer J, Meguerditchian A, Vauclair J, Schapiro SJ, Hopkins WD (2012) Contrast of hemispheric lateralization for oro-facial movements between learned attention-getting sounds and species-typical vocalizations in chimpanzees: extension in a second colony. Brain Lang 123:75–79PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Warneken F, Chen F, Tomasello M (2006) Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Dev 77(3):640–663PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–685PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anna Ilona Roberts
    • 1
    • 2
  • Samuel George Bradley Roberts
    • 1
  • Sarah-Jane Vick
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ChesterChesterUK
  2. 2.Budongo Conservation Field StationMasindiUganda
  3. 3.Psychology, School of Natural SciencesUniversity of StirlingStirlingUK

Personalised recommendations