Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 16, Issue 2, pp 241–254 | Cite as

Information-seeking behavior: exploring metacognitive control in pigeons

  • Leyre Castro
  • Edward A. Wasserman
Original Paper

Abstract

Metacognitive control may occur if an organism seeks additional information when the available information for solving a problem is inadequate. Such information-seeking behavior has been documented in primates, but evidence of analogous behavior is less convincing in non-primates. In our study, we adopted a novel methodological approach. We presented pigeons with visual discriminations of varying levels of difficulty, and on special testing trials, we gave the birds the opportunity of making the discrimination easier. We initially trained pigeons on a discrimination between same and different visual arrays, each containing 12 items (low difficulty), 4 items (intermediate difficulty), or 2 items (high difficulty). We later provided an “Information” button that the pigeons could peck to increase the number of items in the arrays, thereby making the discrimination easier, plus a “Go” button which, when pecked, simply allowed the pigeons to proceed to their final discriminative response. Critically, our pigeons’ choice of the “Information” button increased as the difficulty of the task increased. As well, some of our pigeons showed evidence of prompt and appropriate transfer of using the “Information” button to help them perform brand-new brightness and size discrimination tasks. Speculation as to the contents of pigeons’ private mental states may be unwarranted, but our pigeons did objectively exhibit the kind of complex, flexible, and adaptive information-seeking behavior that is deemed to be involved in metacognitive control.

Keywords

Metacognition Metacognitive control Information-seeking behavior Pigeons 

References

  1. Adams A, Santi A (2011) Pigeons exhibit higher accuracy for chosen memory tests than for forced memory tests in duration matching-to-sample. Learn Behav 39:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s13420-010-0001-7 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Beran MJ, Smith JD (2011) Information seeking by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Cogn 120:90–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bouton ME (1993) Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychol Bull 114:80–99. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Call J (2010) Do apes know that they could be wrong? Anim Cogn 13:689–700PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Call J, Carpenter M (2001) Do apes and children know what they have seen? Anim Cogn 4:207–220. doi: 10.1007/s100710100078 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cantlon JF, Platt M, Brannon EM (2009) Beyond the number domain. Trend Cogn Sci 13:83–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Carruthers P (2008) Meta-cognition in animals: a skeptical look. Mind Lang 23:58–89. doi: org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2007.00329.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Castro L, Wasserman EA (2011) The dimensional nature of same-different discrimination behavior in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 37:361–367. doi: 10.1037/a0021941 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Castro L, Kennedy PL, Wasserman EA (2010) Conditional same-different discrimination by pigeons: acquisition and generalization to novel and few-item displays. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 36:23–38. doi: 10.1037/a0016326 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Castro L, Wasserman EA, Young ME (2012) Variations on variability: effects of display composition on same–different discrimination in pigeons. Learn Behav. Advance online publication. doi:  10.3758/s13420-011-0063-1
  12. Cheng K, Spetch ML, Miceli P (1996) Averaging temporal duration and spatial position. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22:175–182. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.22.2.175 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Crystal JD, Foote AL (2009) Metacognition in animals. Comp Cogn Behav Rev 4:1–16Google Scholar
  14. Crystal JD, Foote AL (2011) Evaluating information-seeking approaches to metacognition. Curr Zool 57:531–542Google Scholar
  15. Dunlosky J, Metcalfe J (2009) Metacognition. Sage Publications, Thousand OaksGoogle Scholar
  16. Foote AL, Crystal JD (2012) “Play it again”: a new method for testing metacognition in animals. Anim Cogn 15:187–199. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0445-y PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fujita K (2009) Metamemory in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 12:575–585. doi: 10.1007/s10071-009-0217-0 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gibson BM, Wasserman EA, Frei L, Miller K (2004) Recent advances in operant conditioning technology: a versatile and affordable computerized touch screen system. Behav Res Meth Instrum Comput 36:355–362. doi: 10.3758/BF03195582 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gunther LM, Denniston JC, Miller RR (1998) Conducting exposure treatment in multiple contexts can prevent relapse. Behav Res Ther 36:75–91. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)10019-5 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hampton RR (2001) Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:5359–5362. doi: 10.1073/pnas.071600998 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hampton RR, Zivin A, Murray EA (2004) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminate between knowing and not knowing and collect information as needed before acting. Anim Cogn 7:239–246. doi: 10.1007/s10071-004-0215-1 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kornell N, Son LK, Terrace HS (2007) Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychol Sci 18:64–71PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Le Pelley ME (2012) Metacognitive monkeys or associative animals? Simple reinforcement learning explains uncertainty in nonhuman animals. J Exp Psyhol Learn Mem Cogn 38:686–708. doi: 10.1037/a0026478 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Logue AW (1988) Research on self-control: an integrating framework. Behav Brain Sci 11:665–679. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00053978 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Marsh HL, MacDonald SE (2011a) Information seeking by orangutans: a generalized search strategy? Anim Cogn 15:293–304. doi: 10.1007/s10071-011-0453-y PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Marsh HL, MacDonald SE (2011b) Orangutans (Pongo abelii) “play the odds”: information-seeking strategies in relation to cost, risk, and benefit. J Comp Psychol. doi: 10.1037/a0025906 PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Nelson TO (1996) Consciousness and metacognition. Am Psychol 51:102–116. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.102 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Nelson TO, Narens L (1990) Metamemory: A theoretical framework and some new findings. In: Bower GH (ed) The psychology of learning and motivation, vol 26. Academic Press, New York, pp 125–173Google Scholar
  29. Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Roberts WA, Feeney MC, McMillan N, Macpherson K, Musolino E (2009) Do pigeons study for a test? J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 35:129–142. doi: 10.1037/a0013722 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Roberts WA, McMillan N, Musolino E, Cole M (2012) Information-seeking in animals: metacognition? Comp Cogn Behav Rev 7:85–109. doi: 10.3819/ccbr.2012.70005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Sarrazin JC, Giraudo MD, Pailhous J, Bootsma RJ (2004) Dynamics of balancing space and time in memory: tau and kappa effects revisited. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:411–430. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.3.411 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Smith SM (1982) Enhancement of recall using multiple environmental contexts during learning. Mem Cogn 10:405–412. doi: 10.3758/BF03197642 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Smith JD, Beran MJ, Couchman JJ, Coutinho MVC (2008) The comparative study of metacognition: sharper paradigms, safer inferences. Psychon Bull Rev 15:679–691. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.679 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tobin H, Logue AW (1994) Self-control across species (Columba livia, Homo sapiens, and Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 108:126–133. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.2.126 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Walsh V (2003) A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trend Cogn Sci 7:483–488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Washburn DA, Smith JD, Shields WE (2006) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) immediately generalize the uncertain response. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 132:185–189. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.32.2.185 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wasserman EA, Young ME (2010) Same-different discrimination: the keel and backbone of thought and reasoning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 36:3–22PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (2012) Comparative cognition: A natural science approach to the study of animal intelligence. In: Zentall TR, Wasserman EA (eds) Oxford handbook of comparative cognition. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Young ME, Wasserman EA (2002) The pigeon’s discrimination of visual entropy: a logarithmic function. Anim Learn Behav 30:306–314PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Young ME, Wasserman EA, Garner KL (1997) Effects of number of items on the pigeon’s discrimination of same from different visual displays. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 23:491–501. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.23.4.491 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Zentall TR, Stagner JP (2010) Pigeons prefer conditional stimuli over their absence: a comment on Roberts et al. (2009). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 36:506–509. doi: 10.1037/a0020202 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2012

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyThe University of IowaIowa CityUSA

Personalised recommendations