Animal Cognition

, 14:695 | Cite as

Relative quantity judgments in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens)

Original Paper

Abstract

There is accumulating evidence that a variety of species possess quantitative abilities although their cognitive substrate is still unclear. This study is the first to investigate whether sea lions (Otaria flavescens), in the absence of training, are able to assess and select the larger of two sets of quantities. In Experiment 1, the two sets of quantities were presented simultaneously as whole sets, that is, the subjects could compare them directly. In Experiment 2, the two sets of quantities were presented item-by-item, and the totality of items was never visually available at the time of choice. For each type of presentation, we analysed the effect of the ratio between quantities, the difference between quantities and the total number of items presented. The results showed that (1) sea lions can make relative quantity judgments successfully and (2) there is a predominant influence of the ratio between quantities on the subjects’ performance. The latter supports the idea that an analogue representational mechanism is responsible for sea lions’ relative quantities judgments. These findings are consistent with previous reports of relative quantities judgments in other species such as monkeys and apes and suggest that sea lions might share a similar mechanism to compare and represent quantities.

Keywords

Numerical cognition Relative quantity judgment Sea lion Comparative cognition Accumulator model 

References

  1. Acevedo J, Aguayo A, Sielfeld W (2003) Eventos reproductivos del león marino común (Otaria flavescens) (Shaw 1800), en el norte de Chile (Pacífico suroriental). Rev Biol Mar Oceanogr 38(2):69–75Google Scholar
  2. Anderson US, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Maple TL (2007) Relative numerousness judgment and summation in Young, middleaged, and older adult Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus ablii and Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 121:1–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baron R, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 61(6):1173–1182Google Scholar
  4. Beran MJ (2001) Summation and numerousness judgments of sequentially presented sets of items by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:181–191PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beran MJ (2004) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to non-visible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. J Comp Psychol 118:25–36PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beran MJ (2007) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) succeed on computerized test designed to assess conservation of discrete quantity. Anim Cogn 10:37–45PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Botham MS, Kerfoot CJ, Louca V, Krause J (2005) Predator choice in the field; grouping guppies, Poecilia reticulate, receive more attacks. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 59:181–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities 1–9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:31–49PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2002) The evolution and ontogeny of ordinal numerical ability. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt GM (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 197–204Google Scholar
  10. Call J (2000) Estimating and operating on discrete quantities in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 114:136–147PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cantlon JF, Brannon EM (2006) Shared system for ordering small and large numbers in monkeys and humans. Psychol Sci 17:401–406PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Capozzo H (2002) South American sea lion (Otaria flavescens). In: Perrin W, Wursig B, Thewissen J (eds) Encyclopedia of marine mammals. Academic Press, New York, pp 1143–1146Google Scholar
  13. Carey S (2001) Cognitive foundations of arithmetic: evolution and ontogenesis. Mind Lang 16:37–55Google Scholar
  14. Davis H, Perusse R (1988) Numerical competence in animals: definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behav Brain Sci 11:561–615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dehaene S (ed) (2001) The cognitive neuroscience of consciousness. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  16. Dooley GB, Gill TV (1977) Acquisition and use of mathematical by a linguistic chimpanzee. In: Rumbaugh DM (ed) Language learning by a chimpanzee: the Lana project. Academic Press, New York, pp 247–260Google Scholar
  17. Evans T, Beran M, Harris E, Rice D (2009) Quantity judgments of sequentially presented food items by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Anim Cogn 12:97–105Google Scholar
  18. Gallistel CR, Gelman R (2000) Non-verbal numerical cognition: from reals and integers. Trends Cogn Sci 4:59–65PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Genty E, Roeder J (2006) Self-control: why should sea lions (Zalophus californianus), perform better than primates? Anim Behav 72:1241–1247CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hanus D, Call J (2007) Discrete quantity judgments in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus): the effect of presenting whole sets versus item-by-item. J Comp Psychol 121:241–249PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hauser MD (2005) Our chimpanzee mind. Nature 437:60–63PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hauser MD, Carey S (1998) Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: evolutionary and developmental insights. In: Cummins D, Allen C (eds) The evolution of mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 51–106Google Scholar
  23. Hauser MD, Spelke E (2004) Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: a case study of mathematics. In: Gazzaniga MS (ed) The cognitive neurosciences III. The MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 853–864Google Scholar
  24. Hauser MD, Carey S, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc Biol Sci 267(1445):829–833PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Irie-Sugimoto N, Kobayashi T, Sato T, Hasegawa T (2009) Relative quantity judgment by Asian Elephants (Elephas maximus). Anim Cogn 12(1):193–199PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jaakkola K, Fellner W, Erb L, Rodriguez M, Guarino E (2005) Understanding of the concept of numerically “less” by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Comp Psychol 119:286–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kahneman D, Treisman A (1984) Changing views of attention and automaticity. In: Parasuraman R, Davies D (eds) Varieties of attention. Academic Press, New York, pp 29–61Google Scholar
  28. Kahneman D, Treisman A, Gibbs BJ (1992) The reviewing of object files: object-specific integration of information. Cogn Psychol 24:174–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kenny DA, Kashy DA, Bolger N (1998) Data analysis in social psychology. In: Gilbert D, Fiske S, Lindzey G (eds) The handbook of social psychology, vol 1, 4th edn. McGraw-Hill, Boston, pp 233–265Google Scholar
  30. Kilian A, Yaman S, von Fersen L, Güntürkün O (2003) A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Learn Behav 31(2):133–142PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kitchen DM, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2004) Factors mediating inter-group encounters in savannah baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Behaviour 141:197–218Google Scholar
  32. Libertus ME, Pruitt LB, Woldorff MG, Brannon EM (2009) Induced alpha-band oscillations reflect ratio-dependent number discrimination in the infant brain. J Cog Neurosci 21(12):2398–2406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. MacKinnon DP, Warsi G, Dwyer JH (1995) A simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multivar Behav Res 30:41–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McComb K, Packer C, Pusey A (1994) Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions (Panthera leo). Anim Behav 47:379–387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Meck WH, Church RM (1983) A mode control model of counting and timing processes. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:320–334PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mix K, Huttenlocher J, Levine S (2002) Multiple cues for quantification in infancy: is number one of them? Psychol Bull 128:278–294PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Pack AA, Herman LM, Roitblat HL (1991) Generalization of visual matching and delayed matching by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Anim Learn Behav 19:37–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Reichmuth Kastak C, Schusterman RJ (2002) Long term memory for concepts in a California sea lion. Anim Cogn 5:225–232PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Schusterman RJ, Reichmuth Kastak C, Kastak D (2002) The cognitive sea lion: meaning and memory in the lab and in nature. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt G (eds) The cognitive animal: empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 217–228Google Scholar
  40. Sepulveda M, Perez MJ, Sielfeld W, Oliva D, Duran LR, Rodriguez L, Araos V, Buscaglia M (2007) Operational interaction between South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens) and artisanal (small-scale) fishing in Chile: results from interview surveys and on-board observations. Fish Res 83(2–3):332–340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Shettleworth S (2009) The evolution of comparative cognition: is the snark still a boojum? Behav Process 80:210–217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Shettleworth S (2010) Cognition, evolution, and behaviour. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  43. Simon TJ, Hespos SJ, Rochat P (1995) Do infants understand arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992). Cogn Dev 10:253–269CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Sobel ME (1982) Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol Methodol 13:290–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Thomas RK (1992) Primates’ conceptual use of number: ecological perspectives and psychological processes. In: Nishida T, McGrew WC, Marler P, Pickfbrd M, de Waal FBM (eds) Topics in primatology, vol. 1: human origins. Tokyo University Press, Tokyo, pp 305–314Google Scholar
  46. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  47. Uller C (2008) Developmental and evolutionary considerations on numerical cognition: a review. J Evol Psychol 6(4):237–253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Uller C, Lewis J (2009) Horses (Equus caballus) select the greater of two quantities in small numerical contrasts. Anim Cogn 12(5):733–738PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Uller C, Carey S, Huntley-Fenner G, Klatt L (1999) What representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge. Cogn Dev 14(1):1–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ward C, Smuts B (2007) Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris). Anim Cogn 10:71–80PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Watts D, Mitani J (2002) Hunting behaviour of chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Int J Primatol 23:1–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wiese H (2003) Iconic and non-iconic stages in number development: the role of language. Trends Cogn Sci 7:385–390PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Wilson M, Hauser M, Wrangham R (2007) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour 144:1621–1653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Xu F, Carey S (1996) Infants’ metaphysics: the case of numerical identity. Cogn Psychol 30:111–153PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Dpto. PsicobiologíaUniversidad Complutense de MadridMadridSpain
  2. 2.Dpto. Metodología de las Ciencias del ComportamientoUniversidad Complutense de MadridMadridSpain
  3. 3.Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary AnthropologyLeipzigGermany

Personalised recommendations