Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 14, Issue 4, pp 607–612 | Cite as

Spatial cognition in zebrafish: the role of strain and rearing environment

  • Rowena Spence
  • Anne E. Magurran
  • Carl Smith
Short Communication

Abstract

Two strains of zebrafish, WIK and a second-generation wild strain were reared in either a structurally simple or complex environment and compared in their ability to locate a food reward in a five-chambered maze. There was a significant interaction within subjects between rearing environment and trial, indicating that the consistency of learning varied depending on rearing environment, with those reared in a structurally simple environment showing a slower rate of learning. Fish of both strains reared in a structurally complex environment were smaller than those reared in a simple environment. Our study demonstrates, for the first time in zebrafish, that performance in a learning task as an adult is sensitive to rearing conditions during development.

Keywords

Cognition Danio rerio Development Environmental enrichment Welfare Learning 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Culum Brown and Martin Reichard for their comments on an earlier version of the manuscript and to Robin Wilson for statistical advice.

References

  1. Basquill SP, Grant JWA (1998) An increase in habitat complexity reduces aggression and monopolization of food by zebra fish (Danio rerio). Can J Zool 76:770–772Google Scholar
  2. Berejikian BA, Smith RJF, Tezak EP, Schroder SL, Knudsen CM (1999) Chemical alarm signals and complex hatchery rearing habitats affect anti-predator behaviour and survival of Chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha) juveniles. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 56:830–838CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bilotta J, Risner ML, Davis EC, Haggbloom SJ (2005) Assessing appetitive choice discrimination learning in zebrafish. Zebrafish 2:251–268CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown C, Braithwaite VA (2004) Effects of predation pressure on the cognitive ability of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcope. Behav Ecol 16:482–487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brown C, Davidson T, Laland K (2003) Environmental enrichment and prior experience of live prey improve foraging behaviour in hatchery-reared Atlantic salmon. J Fish Biol 63:187–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brown C, Laland K, Krause J (2006) Fish cognition and behaviour. In: Brown C, Laland K, Krause J (eds) Fish cognition and behaviour. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 1–8Google Scholar
  7. Burns JG, Rodd H (2008) Hastiness, brain size and predation regime affect the performance of wild guppies in a spatial memory task. Anim Behav 76:911–922CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cameron DA (2002) Mapping absorbance spectra, cone fractions, and neuronal mechanisms to photopic spectral sensitivity in the zebrafish. Vis Neurosci 19:365–372PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Carlier P, Lefebvre L (1997) Ecological differences in social learning between adjacent, mixing, populations of Zanaida doves. Ethology 103:772–784CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Casalini M, Agbali M, Reichard M, Konecna M, Bryjova A, Smith C (2009) Male dominance, female mate choice and intersexual conflict in the rose bitterling (Rhodeus ocellatus). Evolution 63:366–376PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Colwill RM, Raymond MP, Ferreira L, Escudero H (2005) Visual discrimination learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Proc 70:19–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Girvan JR, Braithwaite VA (1998) Population differences in spatial learning in three-spined sticklebacks. Proc R Soc Lond B 265:913–918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Girvan JR, Braithwaite VA (2000) Orientation behaviour in sticklebacks: modified by experience or population specific? Behaviour 137:833–843CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hebb DO (1949) The organisation of behaviour. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  15. Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Gage HF (1997) More hippocampal neurons in mice living in an enriched environment. Nature 386:493–495PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Kieffer JD, Colgan PW (1992) The role of learning in fish behaviour. Rev Fish Biol Fish 2:125–143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lawrence C (2007) The husbandry of zebrafish (Danio rerio): a review. Aquaculture 269:1–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Levin ED, Chen E (2004) Nicotinic involvement in memory function in zebrafish. Neurotoxicol Teratol 26:731–735PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Maynard D, Flagg T, Mahnken C (1995) A review of semi-culture strategies for enhancing the post-release survival of anadromous salmonids. Am Fish Soc Symp 15:307–314Google Scholar
  20. Nieuwenhuys R (1963) The comparative anatomy of the actinopterygian forebrain. J Hirnforsch 13:171–192PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Ninkovic J, Bally-Cuif L (2006) The zebrafish as a model system for assessing the reinforcing properties of drugs of abuse. Methods 39:262–274PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Rodríguez R, López JC, Vargas JP, Gómez Y, Broglio C, Salas C (2002) Conservation of spatial memory function in the pallial forebrain of reptiles and ray-finned fishes. J Neurosci 22:2894–2903PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Rosenzweig MR, Bennet EL (1996) Psychobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behaviour. Behav Brain Res 78:57–65PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Spence R, Smith C (2006) Female mate preferences in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Ecol 17:779–783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Spence R, Fatema MK, Reichard M, Huq KA, Wahab MA, Ahmed ZF, Smith C (2006a) The distribution and habitat preferences of the zebrafish in Bangladesh. J Fish Biol 69:1435–1448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Spence R, Jordan WC, Smith C (2006b) Genetic analysis of male reproductive success in relation to density in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Front Zool 3:5PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Spence R, Ashton RL, Smith C (2007) Adaptive oviposition decisions are mediated by spawning site quality in the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Behaviour 144:953–966CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Spence R, Gerlach G, Lawrence C, Smith C (2008) The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 83:13–34PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Swain HA, Sigstand C, Scalzo FM (2004) Effects of dizocilpine (MK-801) on circling behaviour, swimming activity, and place preference in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Neurotoxicol Teratol 26:725–729PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH (2000) Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:191–198PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (1989) Pigeon homing: olfactory orientation—a paradox. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 24:163–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Wootton RJ (1998) Ecology of teleost fishes. Kluwer, DordrechtGoogle Scholar
  33. Wright D, Rimmer LB, Pritchard VL, Krause J, Butlin RK (2003) Inter and intra-population variation in shoaling and boldness in the zebrafish (Danio rerio). Naturwissenschaften 90:374–377PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wright D, Nakamichi R, Krause J, Butlin RK (2006) QTL analysis of behavioural and morphological differentiation between wild and laboratory zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Genet 36:271–284PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Würbel H (2000) Behaviour and the standardization fallacy. Nat Genet 26:277–281CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of BiologyUniversity of St. AndrewsFifeUK

Personalised recommendations