Animal Cognition

, Volume 14, Issue 3, pp 395–405 | Cite as

Gibbon travel paths are goal oriented

  • Norberto Asensio
  • Warren Y. Brockelman
  • Suchinda Malaivijitnond
  • Ulrich H. Reichard
Original Paper

Abstract

Remembering locations of food resources is critical for animal survival. Gibbons are territorial primates which regularly travel through small and stable home ranges in search of preferred, limited and patchily distributed resources (primarily ripe fruit). They are predicted to profit from an ability to memorize the spatial characteristics of their home range and may increase their foraging efficiency by using a ‘cognitive map’ either with Euclidean or with topological properties. We collected ranging and feeding data from 11 gibbon groups (Hylobates lar) to test their navigation skills and to better understand gibbons’ ‘spatial intelligence’. We calculated the locations at which significant travel direction changes occurred using the change-point direction test and found that these locations primarily coincided with preferred fruit sources. Within the limits of biologically realistic visibility distances observed, gibbon travel paths were more efficient in detecting known preferred food sources than a heuristic travel model based on straight travel paths in random directions. Because consecutive travel change-points were far from the gibbons’ sight, planned movement between preferred food sources was the most parsimonious explanation for the observed travel patterns. Gibbon travel appears to connect preferred food sources as expected under the assumption of a good mental representation of the most relevant sources in a large-scale space.

Keywords

Travel change-point Cognitive map Spatial memory Gibbons Hylobates lar 

Supplementary material

10071_2010_374_MOESM1_ESM.pdf (336 kb)
Supplementary material 1. Diet of the gibbon study groups during observation period (ordered in chronological time). Created with Office 2007. (PDF 335 kb)

References

  1. Barelli C, Heistermann M, Boesch C, Reichard UH (2007) Sexual swellings in wild white-handed gibbon females (Hylobates lar) indicate the probability of ovulation. Horm Behav 51:221–230PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Barelli C, Heistermann M, Boesch C, Reichard UH (2008) Mating patterns and sexual swellings in pair-living and multimale groups of wild white-handed gibbons, Hylobates lar. Anim Behav 75:991–1001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bartlett TQ (1999) Feeding and ranging behaviour of the white-headed gibbon (Hylobates lar) in Khai Yai National Park, Thailand. Ph.D. dissertation thesis, Washington UniversityGoogle Scholar
  4. Bartlett TQ (2007) The Hylobatidae small apes of Asia. In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, MacKinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK (eds) Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 274–289Google Scholar
  5. Bartlett TQ (2009) Seasonal home range use and defendability in white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) in Khao Yai National Park, Thailand. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 265–275Google Scholar
  6. Benhamou S (1996) No evidence for cognitive mapping in rats. Anim Behav 52:201–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Benhamou S (2004) How to reliably estimate the tortuosity of an animal’s path: straightness, sinuosity, or fractal dimension? J Theor Biol 229:209–220PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bennett ATD (1996) Do animals have cognitive maps? J Exp Biol 199:219–224PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Bingman VP, Jones TJ (1994) Sun compass-based spatial learning impaired in homing pigeons with hippocampal lesions. J Neurosci 14:6687–6694PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brockelman WY (1998) Study of tropical forest canopy height and cover using a point-intercept method. In: Dallmeier F, Comiskey JA (eds) Forest biodiversity research, monitoring and modeling: conceptual background and old world case studies. Man and the biosphere series. UNESCO, Paris, and Parthenon Publishing, New York, pp 521–531Google Scholar
  11. Brockelman WY (2009) Ecology and the social system of gibbons. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 211–239Google Scholar
  12. Brockelman WY, Reichard U, Treesucon U, Raemaekers JR (1998) Dispersal, pair formation and social structure in gibbons (Hylobates lar). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:329–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Byrne RW (1979) Memory for urban geography. Q J Exp Psychol 31:147–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Byrne RW (1982) Geographical knowledge and orientation. In: Ellis A (ed) Normality and pathology of cognitive function. Academic Press, London, pp 239–264Google Scholar
  15. Byrne RW (2000) How monkeys find their way: leaderships, coordination, and cognitive maps of African baboons. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. Chicago University Press, Chicago, pp 491–518Google Scholar
  16. Byrne RW, Noser R, Bates LA, Jupp PE (2009) How did they get here from there? Detecting changes of direction in terrestrial ranging. Anim Behav 77:619–631CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cant JGH, Youlatos D, Rose MD (2001) Locomotor behavior of Lagothrix lagotricha and Ateles belzebuth in Yasuni National Park, Ecuador: general patterns and nonsuspensory modes. J Hum Evol 41:141–166PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Clarke E, Reichard UH, Zuberbühler K (2006) The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS ONE 1(1):e73. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000073 PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH (1980) Primate brains and ecology. J Zool 207:151–169Google Scholar
  20. Cunningham EP, Janson CH (2007) Integrating information about location and value of resources by white-faced saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia). Anim Cogn 10:293–304PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Di Fiore A, Campbell CJ (2007) The atelines: variation in ecology, behavior, and social organization. In: Campbell CJ, Fuentes A, Mackinnon KC, Panger M, Bearder SK (eds) Primates in perspective. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 155–185Google Scholar
  22. Di Fiore A, Suarez SA (2007) Route-based travel and shared routes in sympatric spider and woolly monkeys: cognitive and evolutionary implications. Anim Cogn 10:317–329PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dyer FC (1991) Bees acquire route-based memories but not cognitive maps in a familiar landscape. Anim Behav 41:239–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Elder AA (2009) Hylobatid diets revisited: the importance of body mass, fruit availability, and interspecific competition. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 133–159Google Scholar
  25. Erhart EM, Overdorff DJ (2008) Spatial memory during foraging in prosimian primates: Propithecus edwardsi and Eulemur fulvus rufus. Folia Primatol 79:185–196PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Etienne AS, Berlie J, Georgakopoulos J, Maurer R (1998) Role of dead reckoning in navigation. In: Healy S (ed) Spatial representations in animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 54–68Google Scholar
  27. Foo P, Warren WH, Duchon A, Tarr MJ (2005) Do humans integrate routes into a cognitive map? Map- versus landmark-based navigation of novel shortcuts. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 31:195–215PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gallistel CR, Cramer AE (1996) Computations maps in mammals: getting oriented and multidestination route. J Exp Biol 199:211–217PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Garber PA (1989) Role of spatial memory in primate foraging patterns in Saguinus mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis. Am J Primatol 19:203–216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Garber PA (2000) Evidence for the use of spatial, temporal and social information by some primate foragers. In: Boinski S, Garber P (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 165–203Google Scholar
  31. Gillner S, Mallot HA (1997) Navigation and acquisition of spatial knowledge in a virtual maze. J Cog Neurosci 10:445–463CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Gittins SP, Raemaekers JJ (1980) Siamang, lar and agile gibbons. In: Chivers DJ (ed) Malayan forest primates: ten years’ study in tropical rain forest. Plenum Press, New York, pp 63–105Google Scholar
  33. Gould JL (1986) The locale map of honey bees: do insects have cognitive maps? Science 232:861–863PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Held S, Baumgartner J, Kilbride A, Byrne RW (2005) Foraging behaviour in domestic pigs (Sus scrofa): remembering and prioritizing food sites of different value. Anim Cogn 8:114–121PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Hemingway CA, Bynum N (2005) The influence of seasonality on primate diet and ranging. In: Brockman DK, van Schaik CP (eds) Seasonality in primates: studies of living and extinct human and non-human primates. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 57–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Janmaat KRL, Byrne RW, Zuberbühler K (2006) Evidence for a spatial memory of fruiting states of rainforest trees in wild mangabeys. Anim Behav 72:797–807CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Janson CH (1998) Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 55:1229–1243PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Janson CH, Byrne RW (2007) What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box. Anim Cogn 10:357–367PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Janson CH, Di Bitetti MS (1997) Experimental analysis of food detection in capuchin monkeys: effects of distance, travel speed, and resource size. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 41:17–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kim SG, Ugurbil K, Strick PL (1994) Activation of a cerebellar output nucleus during cognitive processing. Science 265:949–951PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Leighton DR (1987) Gibbons: territoriality and monogamy. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (eds) Primate societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 135–145Google Scholar
  42. Link A, Di Fiore A (2006) See dispersal by spider monkeys and its importance in the maintenance of neotropical rain-forest diversity. J Trop Ecol 22:235–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. MacKinnon J (1974) The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Anim Behav 22:3–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. MacLeod CE (2004) What's a brain? The question of a distinctive brain anatomy in great a apes. In: Russon AE, Begun DR (eds) The evolution of thought: evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 105–121Google Scholar
  45. MacLeod CE, Zilles K, Schleicher A, Rilling JK, Gibson KR (2003) Expansion of the neocerebellum in Hominoidea. J Hum Evol 44:401–429PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. McConkey KR (2009) The seed dispersal niche of gibbons in Bornean dipterocarp forests. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 189–207Google Scholar
  47. Menzel R, Brandt R, Gumbert A, Komischke B (2000) Two spatial memories for honeybee navigation. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:961–968CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Milton K (1981) Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to primate mental development. Am Anthropol 83:534–548CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Milton K (2000) Quo vadis? Tactics of food search and group movements in primates and other animals. In: Boinski S, Garber PA (eds) On the move: how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 375–417Google Scholar
  50. Normand E, Boesch C (2009) Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees. Anim Behav 77:1195–1201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Noser RW, Byrne RW (2007) Mental maps in chacma baboons (Papio ursinus): using intergroup encounters as a natural experiment. Anim Cogn 10:331–347PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Noser RW, Byrne RW (2010) How do wild baboons (Papio ursinus) plan their routes? Travel among multiple high-quality food sources with inter-group competition. Anim Cogn 13(1):145–155PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. O’Neill MJ (1991) Evaluation of a conceptual model of architectural legibility. Environ Behav 23:259–284CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Olupot W, Chapman CA, Waser PM, Isabirye Basuta G (1997) Mangabey (Cercocebus albigena) ranging patterns in relation to fruit availability and the risk of parasite infection in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Am J Primatol 43:65–78PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Piaget J, Inhelder B (1956) The child’s conception of space. Routledge, LondonGoogle Scholar
  56. Ponticorvo M, Miglino O (2010) Encoding geometric and non-geometric information: a study with evolved agents. Anim Cogn 13:157–174PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Potts R (2004) Paleoenvironmental basis of cognitive evolution in great apes. Am J Primatol 62:209–238PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Poucet B (1993) Spatial cognitive maps in animals, new hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms. Psychol Rev 100:163–182PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Reichard UH (2003) Social monogamy in gibbons: the male perspective. In: Reichard UH, Boesch C (eds) Monogamy: mating strategies and partnerships in birds, humans and other mammals. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 190–213Google Scholar
  60. Reichard UH (2009) The social organization and mating system of Khao Yai white-handed gibbons: 1992–2006. In: Lappan S, Whittacker DJ (eds) The gibbons: new perspectives on small ape socioecology and population biology. Springer, New York, pp 347–384Google Scholar
  61. Reichard U, Sommer V (1997) Group encounters in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar): agonism, affiliation, and the concept of infanticide. Behaviour 134:1135–1174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Rilling JK, Insel TR (1998) Evolution of the primate cerebellum: differences in relative volume among monkeys, apes and humans. Brain Behav Evol 52:308–314PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Savini T, Boesch C, Reichard UH (2008) Home range characteristics and the influence of seasonality on females reproductive success in white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar) at Khao Yai National Park, Thailand. Am J Phys Anthropol 135:1–12PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Schkolkopf B, Mallot HA (1995) View-based cognitive mapping and path planning. Adapt Behav 3:311–348CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Schoener TW (1983) Simple models of optimal feeding territory size: a reconciliation. Am Nat 121:608–629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Shanahan M, So S, Compton SG, Corlett RT (2001) Fig-eating by vertebrate frugivores: a global review. Biol Rev 76:529–572PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Shettleworth SJ (1998) Cognition, evolution, and behavior. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  68. Silverman I (2006) Non-Euclidean navigational strategies of women: compensatory response or evolved dimorphism? Evol Psychol 4:75–84Google Scholar
  69. Silverman I, Choi J (2005) Locating places. In: Buss DM (ed) The evolutionary psychology handbook. Wiley, Hoboken, pp 177–199Google Scholar
  70. Sturz BR, Bodily KD, Katz JS (2006) Evidence against integration of spatial maps in humans. Anim Cogn 9:207–217PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Tolman EC (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychol Rev 55:198–208Google Scholar
  72. Valero A, Byrne RW (2007) Spider monkey ranging patterns in Mexican subtropical forest: do travel routes reflect planning? Anim Cogn 10:305–331PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. van Schaik CP, Terborgh JW, Wright SJ (1993) The phenology of tropical forests: adaptive significance and the consequences for primary consumers. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 24:353–377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Wallraff H (2001) Navigation by homing pigeons, part 2. Ethol Ecol Evol 1–48Google Scholar
  75. Wehner R (1981) Spatial vision in arthropods. In: Autrum H (ed) Handbook of sensory physiology, vol VII/6C. Springer, Berlin, pp 287–616Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Norberto Asensio
    • 1
  • Warren Y. Brockelman
    • 1
  • Suchinda Malaivijitnond
    • 2
  • Ulrich H. Reichard
    • 3
  1. 1.Conservation Genetics and Ecology Group, Institute of Molecular BiosciencesMahidol UniversityNakhorn PathonThailand
  2. 2.Primate Research Unit, Department of Biology, Faculty of ScienceChulalongkorn UniversityBangkokThailand
  3. 3.Department of AnthropologySouthern Illinois UniversityCarbondaleUSA

Personalised recommendations