Animal Cognition

, Volume 13, Issue 5, pp 721–731

Stereotyping starlings are more ‘pessimistic’

Original Paper

Abstract

Negative affect in humans and animals is known to cause individuals to interpret ambiguous stimuli pessimistically, a phenomenon termed ‘cognitive bias’. Here, we used captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) to test the hypothesis that a reduction in environmental conditions, from enriched to non-enriched cages, would engender negative affect, and hence ‘pessimistic’ biases. We also explored whether individual differences in stereotypic behaviour (repetitive somersaulting) predicted ‘pessimism’. Eight birds were trained on a novel conditional discrimination task with differential rewards, in which background shade (light or dark) determined which of two covered dishes contained a food reward. The reward was small when the background was light, but large when the background was dark. We then presented background shades intermediate between those trained to assess the birds’ bias to choose the dish associated with the smaller food reward (a ‘pessimistic’ judgement) when the discriminative stimulus was ambiguous. Contrary to predictions, changes in the level of cage enrichment had no effect on ‘pessimism’. However, changes in the latency to choose and probability of expressing a choice suggested that birds learnt rapidly that trials with ambiguous stimuli were unreinforced. Individual differences in performance of stereotypies did predict ‘pessimism’. Specifically, birds that somersaulted were more likely to choose the dish associated with the smaller food reward in the presence of the most ambiguous discriminative stimulus. We propose that somersaulting is part of a wider suite of behavioural traits indicative of a stress response to captive conditions that is symptomatic of a negative affective state.

Keywords

Sturnus vulgaris European starling Stereotypic behaviour Cognitive bias Environmental enrichment Anxiety 

References

  1. Asher L (2007) Welfare of captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Ph.D. Thesis. Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon TyneGoogle Scholar
  2. Asher L, Davies GTO, Bertenshaw CE, Cox MAA, Bateson M (2009) The effects of cage volume and cage shape on the condition and behaviour of captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Appl Anim Behav Sci 116:286–294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van Ijendoorn MH (2007) Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Bull 133:1–24CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bateson M, Kacelnik A (1995) Preferences for fixed and variables food sources–variability in amount and delay. J Exp Anal Behav 63:313–329CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Bateson M, Matheson SM (2007) Performance on a categorisation task suggests that removal of environmental enrichment induces ‘pessimism’ in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Anim Welfare 16:33–36Google Scholar
  6. Bergvall UA, Rautio P, Luotola T, Leimar O (2007) A test of simultaneous and successive negative contrast in fallow deer foraging behaviour. Anim Behav 74:395–402CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blumstein DT, Evans CS, Daniel JC (2000) J Watcher 0.9. An introductory user’s guideGoogle Scholar
  8. Brilot BO, Asher L, Feenders G, Bateson M (2009a) Quantification of abnormal repetitive behavior in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Behav Process 82:256–264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brilot BO, Normandale CL, Parkin A, Bateson M (2009b) Can we use starlings’ aversion to eyespots as the basis for a novel ‘cognitive bias’ task? Appl Anim Behav Sci 118:182–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Burman OHP, Parker R, Paul ES, Mendl M (2008a) A spatial judgement task to determine background emotional state in laboratory rats, Rattus norvegicus. Anim Behav 76:801–809CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Burman OHP, Parker RMA, Paul ES, Mendl M (2008b) Sensitivity to reward loss as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare. Biol Lett 4:330–333CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Enkel T, Gholizadeh D, von Bohlen Und Halbach O, Sanchis-Segura C, Hurlemann R, Spanagel R, Gass P, Vollmayr B (2009) Ambiguous-cue interpretation is biased under stress- and depression-like states in rats. Neuropsychopharmacol. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.204
  13. Eysenck MW, Mogg K, May J, Richards A, Mathews A (1991) Bias in interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety. J Abnorm Psychol 100:144–150CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Flaherty CF (1996) Incentive relativity. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  15. Greenwood VJ, Smith EL, Goldsmith AR, Cuthill IC, Crisp LH, Walter-Swan MB, Bennett ATD (2004) Does the flicker frequency of fluorescent lighting affect the welfare of captive European starlings? Appl Anim Behav Sci 86:145–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Harding EJ, Paul ES, Mendl M (2004) Animal behavior: cognitive bias and affective state. Nature 427:312CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Maddocks SA, Goldsmith AR, Cuthill IC (2002) Behavioural and physiological effects of absence of ultraviolet wavelengths on European starlings Sturnus vulgaris. J Avian Biol 33:103–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Mason G (2006) Stereotypic behaviour in captive animals: fundamentals and implications for welfare and beyond. In: Mason G, Rushen J (eds) Stereotypic animal behaviour: fundamentals and applications to welfare, 2nd edn. CABI, Wallingford, pp 325–356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Mason GJ, Latham NR (2004) Can’t stop, won’t stop: is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Anim Welfare 13:S57–S69Google Scholar
  20. Mason G, Rushen J (2006) Stereotypic animal behaviour: fundamentals and applications to welfare, 2nd edn. CABI, WallingfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Matheson SM (2007) Using cognitive bias to assess welfare in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). M.Sc. Thesis. Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon TyneGoogle Scholar
  22. Matheson SM, Asher L, Bateson M (2008) Larger, enriched cages are associated with ‘optimistic’ response biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Appl Anim Behav Sci 109:374–383CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mendl M (1999) Performing under pressure: stress and cognitive function. Appl Anim Behav Sci 65:221–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Mendl M, Burman OHP, Parker RMA, Paul ES (2009) Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl Anim Behav Sci 118:161–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Okuda S, Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL (2004) Glucocorticoid effects on object recognition memory require training-associated emotional arousal. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:853–858CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M (2005) Measuring emotional processes in animals: the utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:469–491CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Rich EL, Romero LM (2005) Exposure to chronic stress downregulates corticosterone responses to acute stressors. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 288:R1628–R1636PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Williams JMG, Watts FN, MacLeod C, Matthews A (1997) Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders. Wiley, ChichesterGoogle Scholar
  29. Young RJ (2003) Environmental enrichment for captive animals. Blackwell Publishing, OxfordCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Behaviour and Evolution, Institute of NeuroscienceNewcastle UniversityNewcastle upon TyneUK
  2. 2.Department of Veterinary Clinical SciencesRoyal Veterinary CollegeHertfordshireUK

Personalised recommendations