Animal Cognition

, Volume 13, Issue 5, pp 771–776 | Cite as

The memory remains: long-term vocal recognition in Australian sea lions

  • Benjamin J. Pitcher
  • Robert G. Harcourt
  • Isabelle Charrier
Short Communication

Abstract

The ability to recognize other individuals plays an important role in mediating social interactions. As longitudinal studies are challenging, there is only limited evidence of long-term memory of individuals and concepts in mammals. We examined the ability of six wild Australian sea lions to discriminate between the voice of their mother and another adult female, both while they were dependent on their mother and when they were independent, 2 years after weaning. Here, we show that even after a long period of independence, juveniles retain the ability to identify their mother’s voice. Both when dependent and independent, animals showed stronger responses to maternal calls than to the calls of another female. This demonstration of recognition provides rare evidence of the long-term memory capabilities of wild mammals.

Keywords

Long-term memory Individual vocal recognition Neophoca cinerea Discrimination Social communication Pinniped 

References

  1. Acevedo-Whitehouse K, Gulland F, Greig D, Amos W (2003) Disease susceptibility in California sea lions. Nature 422:35CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Boughman JW, Moss CF (2002) Social sounds: vocal learning and development of mammal and bird calls. In: Simmons AM, Popper AN, Fay RR (eds) Acoustic communication. Springer, New York, pp 138–224Google Scholar
  3. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of animal communication. Sinauer Associates, SunderlandGoogle Scholar
  4. Campbell RA, Gales NJ, Lento GM, Baker CS (2008) Islands in the sea: extreme female natal site fidelity in the Australian sea lion, Neophoca cinerea. Biol Lett 4:139–142CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Chalmers N (1983) The development of social relationships. In: Halliday TR, Slater PJB (eds) Animal behaviour, vol 3. Genes, development and learning. Blackwell Scientific Publications, London, pp 114–148Google Scholar
  6. Charrier I, Harcourt RG (2006) Individual vocal identity in mother and pup Australian sea lions (Neophoca cinerea). J Mammal 87:929–938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Charrier I, Mathevon N, Jouventin P (2001) Mother’s voice recognition by seal pups—newborns need to learn their mother’s call before she can take off on a fishing trip. Nature 412:873Google Scholar
  8. Charrier I, Mathevon N, Jouventin P (2003) Fur seal mothers memorize subsequent versions of developing pups’ calls: adaptation to long-term recognition or evolutionary by-product? Biol J Linn Soc 80:305–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Charrier I, Pitcher BJ, Harcourt RG (2009) Vocal recognition of mothers by Australian sea lion pups: individual signature and environmental constraints. Anim Behav doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.032
  10. Fabiani A, Galimberti F, Sanvito S, Hoelzel R (2006) Relatedness and site fidelity at the southern elephant seal, Mirounga leonina, breeding colony in the Falkland Islands. Anim Behav 72:617–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hanggi EB (1992) The importance of vocal cues in mother-pup recognition in a California sea lion. Mar Mamm Sci 8:430–432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Hanggi EB, Ingersoll JF (2009) Long-term memory for categories and concepts in horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 12:451–462CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Hanggi EB, Schusterman RJ (1990) Kin recognition in captive California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). J Comp Psychol 104:368–372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Higgins LV, Gass L (1993) Birth to weaning: parturition, duration of lactation, and attendance cycles of Australian sea lions (Neophoca cinerea). Can J Zool 71:2047–2055CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hoffman JI, Forcada J, Trathan PN, Amos W (2007) Female fur seals show active choice for males that are heterozygous and unrelated. Nature 445:912–914CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Insley SJ (2000) Long-term vocal recognition in the northern fur seal. Nature 406:404–405CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Insley SJ, Phillips AV, Charrier I (2003) A review of social recognition in pinnipeds. Aquat Mamm 29:181–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kendrick KM, da Costa AP, Leigh AE, Hinton MR, Peirce JW (2001) Sheep don’t forget a face. Nature 414:165–166CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Marlow PJ (1972) Pup abduction in the Australian sea lion, Neophoca cinerea. Mammalia 36:161–165Google Scholar
  20. Marlow PJ (1975) The comparative behaviour of the Australasian sea lions Neophoca cinerea and Phocarctos hookeri (Pinnipedia: Otariidae). Mammalia 39:159–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McComb K, Moss C, Sayialel S, Baker L (2000) Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Anim Behav 59:1103–1109CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. McGregor PK (1992) Quantifying responses to playback: one, many, or composite multivariate measures. In: McGregor PK (ed) Playback and studies of animal communication. Plenum, New York, pp 79–96Google Scholar
  23. McIntosh RR (2007) Life history and population demographics of the Australian sea lion. PhD Thesis, La Trobe UniversityGoogle Scholar
  24. Newberry RC, Swanson JC (2008) Implications of breaking mother–young social bonds. Appl Anim Behav Sci 110:3–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Pitcher BJ, Ahonen H, Harcourt RG, Charrier I (2009) Delayed onset of vocal recognition in Australian sea lion pups (Neophoca cinerea). Naturwissenschaften 96:901–909CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Poland VF, Pomeroy PP, Twiss SD, Graves JA (2008) Fine-scale study finds limited evidence of kin clustering in a gray seal colony. Mar Mamm Sci 24:371–387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Pomeroy PP, Twiss SD, Redman P (2000) Philopatry, site fidelity and local kin associations within grey seal breeding colonies. Ethology 106:899–919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Reichmuth Kastak C, Schusterman RJ (2002) Long-term memory for concepts in a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Anim Cogn 5:225–232CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Ross KG (2001) Molecular ecology of social behaviour: analysis of breeding systems and genetic structure. Mol Ecol 10:265–284CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Schusterman RJ, Hanggi EB, Gisiner R (1992) Acoustic signalling in mother-pup reunions, interspecies bonding, and affiliation by kinship in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). In: Thomas J, Kastelein RA, Supin YA (eds) Marine mammal sensory systems. Plenum Press, New York, pp 533–551Google Scholar
  31. Shaughnessy PD (1999) The action plan for Australian seals. Environment Australia, CanberraGoogle Scholar
  32. Stirling I (1972) Observations on the Australian sea lion, Neophoca cinerea (Peron). Aust J Zool 20:271–279CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Trillmich F (1981) Mutual mother–pup recognition in Galápagos fur seals and sea lions: cues used and functional significance. Behaviour 78:21–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Wolf JBW, Trillmich F (2008) Kin in space: social viscosity in a spatially and genetically substructured network. Proc R Soc B 275:2063–2069CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Wolf JBW, Mawdsley D, Trillmich F, James R (2007) Social structure in a colonial mammal: unraveling hidden structural layers and their foundations by network analysis. Anim Behav 74:1293–1302CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  • Benjamin J. Pitcher
    • 1
    • 2
  • Robert G. Harcourt
    • 1
  • Isabelle Charrier
    • 2
  1. 1.Marine Mammal Research Group, Graduate School of the EnvironmentMacquarie UniversitySydneyAustralia
  2. 2.Equipe Communications AcoustiquesCNPS, CNRS, Université Paris SudOrsayFrance

Personalised recommendations