Animal Cognition

, Volume 13, Issue 1, pp 33–40

Multimodal communication by captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

  • David A. Leavens
  • Jamie L. Russell
  • William D. Hopkins
Original Paper

Abstract

Many studies have shown that apes and monkeys are adept at cross-modal matching tasks requiring the subject to identify objects in one modality when information regarding those objects has been presented in a different modality. However, much less is known about non-human primates’ production of multimodal signaling in communicative contexts. Here, we present evidence from a study of 110 chimpanzees demonstrating that they select the modality of communication in accordance with variations in the attentional focus of a human interactant, which is consistent with previous research. In each trial, we presented desirable food to one of two chimpanzees, turning mid-way through the trial from facing one chimpanzee to facing the other chimpanzee, and documented their communicative displays, as the experimenter turned towards or away from the subjects. These chimpanzees varied their signals within a context-appropriate modality, displaying a range of different visual signals when a human experimenter was facing them and a range of different auditory or tactile (attention-getting) signals when the human was facing away from them; this finding extends previous research on multimodal signaling in this species. Thus, in the impoverished circumstances characteristic of captivity, complex signaling tactics are nevertheless exhibited by chimpanzees, suggesting continuity in intersubjective psychological processes in humans and apes.

Keywords

Chimpanzee Communication Multimodal Gesture Intentional communication Elaboration 

References

  1. Bard KA (1998) Social-experiential contributions to imitation and emotion in chimpanzees. In: Bråten S (ed) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny: a source book. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 208–227Google Scholar
  2. Bard KA, Leavens DA (2009) Socio-emotional factors in the development of joint attention in human and ape infants. In: Roska-Hardy L, Neumann-Held EM (eds) Learning from animals? Examining the nature of human uniqueness. Psychology Press, London, pp 89–104Google Scholar
  3. Bates E, Camaioni L, Volterra V (1975) Performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quart 21:205–226Google Scholar
  4. Brinck I (2001) Attention and the evolution of intentional communication. Pragmat Cogn 9:255–272Google Scholar
  5. Call J, Tomasello M (1994) Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 108:307–317CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Camaioni L, Perucchini P, Bellagamba F, Colonnesi C (2004) The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy 5:291–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cartmill EA, Byrne RW (2007) Orangutans modify their gestural signaling according to their audience’s comprehension. Curr Biol 17:1345–1348CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cowey A, Weiskrantz L (1975) Demonstration of cross-modal matching in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Neuropsychologia 13:117–120CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Davenport RK (1977) Cross-modal perception: a basis for language? In: Rumbaugh DM (ed) Language learning by a chimpanzee: the LANA project. Academic Press, New York, pp 73–83Google Scholar
  10. Davenport RK, Rogers CM (1970) Intermodal equivalence of stimuli in apes. Science 168:279–280CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Davenport RK, Rogers CM (1971) Perception of photographs by apes. Behaviour 39:2–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Davenport RK, Rogers CM, Russell IS (1973) Cross-modal perception in apes. Neuropsychologia 11:21–28CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Doherty M (2006) The development of mentalistic gaze understanding. Infant Child Dev 15:179–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ettlinger G (1960) Cross-modal transfer of training in monkeys. Behaviour 16:56–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Ettlinger G, Jarvis MJ (1976) Cross-modal transfer in a chimpanzee. Nature 259:44–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Forrester GS (2008) A multidimensional approach to investigations of behaviour: revealing structure in animal communication signals. Anim Behav 76:1749–1760CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Friedes D (1974) Human information processing in the sensory modalities: cross-modal functions, information complexity, memory and deficit. Psychol Bull 81:284–310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Geschwind N (1965) Disconnection syndrome in animals and man. Brain 88:237–294CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Golinkoff RM (1986) ‘I beg your pardon?’: the preverbal negotiation of failed messages. J Child Lang 13:455–476CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Golinkoff RM (1993) When is communication a ‘meeting of minds’? J Child Lang 20:199–207CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Gómez J-C (1998) Do concepts of intersubjectivity apply to non-human primates? In: Bråten S (ed) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny: a source book. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 245–259Google Scholar
  22. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns in adaptation. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  23. Hayes KJ, Hayes C (1951) The intellectual development of a home-raised chimpanzee. Proc Am Philos Soc 95:105–109Google Scholar
  24. Hopkins WD, Taglialatela JP, Leavens DA (2007) Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Anim Behav 73:281–286CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Hostetter AB, Cantero M, Hopkins WD (2001) Differential use of vocal and gestural communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in response to the attentional status of a human (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 115:337–343PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hostetter AB, Russell JL, Freeman H, Hopkins WD (2007) Now you see me, now you don’t: evidence that chimpanzees understand the role of the eyes in attention. Anim Cogn 10:55–62CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Kellogg WN (1968) Communication and language in the home-raised chimpanzee. Science 162:423–427CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Krause MA, Fouts RS (1997) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) pointing: hand shapes, accuracy, and the role of eye gaze. J Comp Psychol 111:330–336CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Leavens DA (2004) Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interact Stud 5:387–408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Leavens DA (2006) It takes time and experience to learn how to interpret gaze in mentalistic terms. Infant Child Dev 9:187–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD (1998) Intentional communication by chimpanzees: a cross-sectional study of the use of referential gestures. Dev Psychol 34:813–822CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD (2005) Multimodal concomitants of manual gesture by chimpanzees: effects of food size and distance. Gesture 5:73–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD, Bard KA (1996) Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 110:346–353CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD, Thomas RK (2004a) Referential communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 118:48–57CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Leavens DA, Hostetter AB, Wesley MJ, Hopkins WD (2004b) Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim Behav 67:467–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD, Bard KA (2005a) Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing: epigenesis and ecological validity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 14:185–189CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Leavens DA, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2005b) Intentionality as measured in the persistence and elaboration of communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Child Dev 76:291–306CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD, Bard KA (2008) The heterochronic origins of explicit reference. In: Zlatev J, Racine TP, Sinha C, Itkonen E (eds) The shared mind: perspectives on intersubjectivity. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 187–214Google Scholar
  39. Leavens DA, Racine TP, Hopkins WD (2009) The ontogeny and phylogeny of non-verbal deixis. In: Botha R, Knight C (eds) The prehistory of language. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 142–165Google Scholar
  40. Liebal K, Pika S, Call J, Tomasello M (2004) To move or not to move: how apes adjust to the attentional state of others. Interact Stud 5:199–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Henning A, Striano T, Tomasello M (2004) Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Dev Sci 7:297–307CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Moore C, Corkum V (1994) Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Dev Rev 14:349–372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. O’Neill DK (1996) Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Dev 67:659–677CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Partan SR (2002) Single and multichannel signal composition: facial expression and vocalizations of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behaviour 139:993–1027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Povinelli DJ, Eddy TJ (1996) What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 62(3, Serial no. 247), pp 1–152Google Scholar
  46. Racine TP, Carpendale JIM (2007) The role of shared practice in joint attention. Br J Dev Psychol 15:3–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Racine TP, Leavens DA, Susswein N, Wereha TJ (2008) Pointing as intersubjectivity in human and nonhuman primates. In: Morganti F, Carassa A, Riva G (eds) Enacting intersubjectivity: a cognitive and social perspective to the study of interactions. IOS Press, Amsterdam, pp 65–79Google Scholar
  48. Russell JL, Braccini S, Buehler N, Kachin MJ, Schapiro SJ, Hopkins WD (2005) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) intentional communication is not contingent upon food. Anim Cogn 8:263–274CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Sevcik RA, Hopkins WD (1988) Symbolic cross-modal transfer in two species of chimpanzees. Child Dev 59:617–625CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Tolan J, Rogers CM, Malone DR (1981) Cross-modal matching in monkeys: altered visual cues and delay. Neuropsychologia 19:289–300CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  52. Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1994) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 35:137–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Trevarthen C, Hubley P (1978) Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture and symbol. Academic Press, New York, pp 183–229Google Scholar
  54. Veà JJ, Sabater-Pi J (1998) Spontaneous pointing behaviour in wild pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Folia Primatol 69:289–290CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Ward JP, Yehle AL, Doerflein RS (1970) Cross-modal transfer of a specific discrimination in the bush baby (Galago senegalensis). J Comp Physiol Psychol 73:74–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • David A. Leavens
    • 1
  • Jamie L. Russell
    • 2
  • William D. Hopkins
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of Psychology, School of Life SciencesUniversity of SussexEast SussexUK
  2. 2.Division of PsychobiologyYerkes National Primate Research CenterAtlantaUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyAgnes Scott CollegeDecaturUSA

Personalised recommendations