Animal Cognition

, Volume 12, Issue 4, pp 621–631

A comparative approach to dogs’ (Canis familiaris) and human infants’ comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures

  • Gabriella Lakatos
  • Krisztina Soproni
  • Antal Dóka
  • Ádám Miklósi
Original Paper

Abstract

We investigated whether dogs and 2-, and 3-year-old human infants living, in some respects, in very similar social environments are able to comprehend various forms of the human pointing gesture. In the first study, we looked at their ability to comprehend different arm pointing gestures (long cross-pointing, forward cross-pointing and elbow cross-pointing) to locate a hidden object. Three-year-olds successfully used all gestures as directional cues, while younger children and dogs could not understand the elbow cross-pointing. Dogs were also unsuccessful with the forward cross-pointing. In the second study, we used unfamiliar pointing gestures i.e. using a leg as indicator (pointing with leg, leg cross-pointing, pointing with knee). All subjects were successful with leg pointing gestures, but only older children were able to comprehend the pointing with knee. We suggest that 3-year-old children are able to rely on the direction of the index finger, and show the strongest ability to generalize to unfamiliar gestures. Although some capacity to generalize is also evident in younger children and dogs, especially the latter appear biased in the use of protruding body parts as directional signals.

Keywords

Dogs Children Communication Human gestures Pointing 

References

  1. Albert A, Bulcroft K (1987) Pets and urban life. Anthrozoos 1:9–25Google Scholar
  2. Albert A, Bulcroft K (1988) Pets, families, and the life course. J Marriage Fam 50:543–552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Allan LG (1993) Human contingency learning: rule based or associative? Psychol Bull 114:435–448PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anderson JR, Sallaberry P, Barbier H (1995) Use of experimenter-given cues during object choice tasks by capuchin monkeys. Anim Behav 49:201–208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Behne T, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2005) One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game. Dev Sci 8:492–499PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog–causal ape. J Comp Psychol 120:38–47PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brownell CA, Ramani GB, Zerwas S (2006) Becoming a social partner with peers: cooperation and social understanding in one- and two-year-olds. Child Dev 77:803–821PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Butterworth GE, Grover L (1998) Joint visual attention, manual pointing, and preverbal communication in human infancy. In: Jeannerod M, Hillsdale NJ (eds) Attention and performance. Erlbaum, HillsdaleGoogle Scholar
  9. Cain AO (1985) Pet as family members. In: Sussman A (ed) Pets and the family. The Haworth Press, New York, pp 5–10Google Scholar
  10. Call J, Hare B, Tomasello M (1998) Chimpanzee gaze following in an object choice task. Anim Cogn 1:89–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Call J, Agnetta B, Tomasello M (2000) Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Anim Cogn 3:23–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dobrich L, Scarborough A (1984) Form and function in early communication. Language and pointing gestures. J Exp Child Psychol 38:475–490PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Evans CS (1997) Referential signals. In: Owings DH, Beecher MD, Thompson NS (eds) Perspectives in ethology, communication, vol 12. Plenum Press, New York, pp 99–143Google Scholar
  14. Frank H (1980) Evolution of canine information processing under conditions of natural and artificial selection. Z Tierpsychol 53:389–399PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Gácsi M, Győri B, Miklósi Á, Virányi Z, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Csányi V (2005) Species-specific differences and similarities in the behavior of hand raised dog and wolf puppies in social situations with humans. Dev Psychobiol 47:111–122PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gácsi M, Kara E, Belényi B, Topál J, Miklósi Á (2009) The effect of development and individual differences on pointing comprehension in. Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0208-6)
  17. Hare B, Tomasello M (1999) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human and conspecific social cues to locate hidden food. J Comp Psychol 113:173–177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Herman LM, Abichandani SL, Elhajj AN, Herman EYK, Sanchez JL, Pack AA (1999) Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referential character of the human pointing gesture. J Comp Psychol 113:347–364PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hirsch-Pasek K, Treiman R (1981) Doggerel: motherese in a new context. J Child Lang 9:229–237Google Scholar
  21. Itakura S, Tanaka M (1998) Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 112:119–126PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jenkins WO (1943) A spatial factor in chimpanzee learning. J Comp Psychol 35:81–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD (1999) The whole-hand pointing: the structure and function of pointing from a comparative perspective. J Comp Psychol 113:417–425PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Leung EHL, Rheingold HL (1981) Development of pointing as a social gesture. Dev Psychol 17:215–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McKinley J, Sambrook TD (2000) Use of human-given cues by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and horses (Equus caballus). Anim Cogn 3:13–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Miklósi Á, Soproni K (2006) A comparative analysis of the animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim Cogn 9:81–93PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Miklósi Á, Polgárdi R, Topál J, Csányi V (1998) Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Anim Cogn 1:113–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Gácsi M, Virányi Z, Csányi V (2003) A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans but dogs do. Curr Biol 13:763–766PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2004) Comparative social cognition: what can dogs teach us? Anim Behav 67:995–1004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Miklósi Á, Pongrácz P, Lakatos G, Topál J, Csányi V (2005) A comparative study of the use of visual comminicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. J Comp Psychol 119:179–186PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Milgram NW, Adams B, Callahan H, Head E, Mackay B, Thirlwell C, Cotman CW (1999) Landmark discrimination learning in the dog. Learn Memory 6:54–61Google Scholar
  32. Mitchell RW (2001) Americans’ talk to dogs: similarities and differences with talk to infants. Res Lang Soc Interact 34:183–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Morisette P, Ricard M, Decarie TG (1995) Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy: a longitudinal study of comprehension. Br J Dev Psychol 13:163–175Google Scholar
  34. Murphy JV, Miller RE (1955) The effect of spatial contiguity of cue and reward in the object-quality learning of rhesus monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol 48:221–224PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Pongrácz P, Miklósi A, Csányi V (2001) Owners’ beliefs on the ability of their pet dogs to understand human verbal communication. A case of social understanding. Curr Cogn Psychol 20:87–107Google Scholar
  36. Povinelli DJ, Reaux JE, Bierschwale DT, Allain AD, Simon BB (1997) Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cogn Dev 12:423–461CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Povinelli DJ, Bierschwale DT, Cech CG (1999) Comprehension of seeing as a referential act in young children, but not juvenile chimpanzees. Br J Dev Psychol 17:37–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Prato-Previde E, Fallani G, Valsecchi P (2006) Gender differences in owners interacting with pet dogs: an observational study. Ethology 112:64–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Rasmussen JL, Rajecki DW (1995) Differences and similarities in humans’ perceptions of the thinking and feeling of a dog and a boy. Soc Anim 3:117–137CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Scheumann M, Call J (2004) The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Anim Cogn 7:224–230PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Shapiro AD, Janik VM, Slater PJB (2004) A grey seal’s (Halichoerus grypus) responses to experimenter-given pointing and directional cues. J Comp Psychol 117:355–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2001) Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris). J Comp Psychol 115:122–126PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Soproni K, Miklósi Á, Topál J, Csányi V (2002) Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. J Comp Psychol 116:27–34PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Tomasello M, Camaioni L (1997) A comparison of the gestural communication of apes and human infants. Hum Dev 40:7–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Tomasello M, Call J, Gluckman A (1997) Comprehension of novel communicative signs by apes and human children. Child Dev 68:1067–1080PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav Brain Sci 28(5):675–691PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Topál J, Miklósi Á, Csányi V, Dóka A (1998) Attachment behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris): a new application of Ainsworth’s (1969) strange situation test. J Comp Psychol 112:219–229PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tschudin A, Call J, Dunbar RIM, Harris G, van der Elst C (2001) Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Comp Psychol 115:100–105PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Virányi Zs, Gácsi M, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Belényi B, Ujfalussy D, Miklósi Á (2008) Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves and dogs. Anim Cogn 11:373–387PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Wilkins D (2003) Why pointing with the index finger is not a universal (in sociocultural and semiotic terms). In: Kita S (ed) Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 171–215Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gabriella Lakatos
    • 1
  • Krisztina Soproni
    • 2
  • Antal Dóka
    • 1
  • Ádám Miklósi
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of EthologyEötvös University BudapestBudapestHungary
  2. 2.Department of Behavioural Neurobiology, Institute of Experimental Medicine Hungarian Academy of SciencesBudapestHungary

Personalised recommendations