Animal Cognition

, Volume 11, Issue 4, pp 661–674 | Cite as

Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use

Original Paper

Abstract

Planning for future needs has traditionally been considered to be restricted to human cognition. Although recent studies on great ape and corvid cognition challenge this belief, the phylogenesis of human planning remains largely unknown. The complex skill for future planning has not yet been satisfactorily established in any other extant primate species than our own. In humans, planning for future needs rely heavily on two overarching capacities, both of which lie at the heart of our cognition: self-control, often defined as the suppression of immediate drives in favor of delayed rewards, and mental time travel, which could be described as a detached mental experience of a past or future event. Future planning is linked to additional high complexity cognition such as metacognition and a consciousness usually not attributed to animals. In a series of four experiments based on tool use, we demonstrate that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and orangutans (Pongo abelii) override immediate drives in favor of future needs, and they do not merely rely on associative learning or semantic prospection when confronted with a planning task. These results suggest that great apes engage in planning for the future by out competing current drives and mentally pre-experiencing an upcoming event. This suggests that the advanced mental capacities utilized in human future planning are shared by phylogenetically more ancient species than previously believed.

Keywords

Planning Mental time travel Self-control Bischof–Köhler-hypothesis Animal consciousness 

References

  1. Ainslie G (1974) Impulse control in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav 21:485–489PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ainslie G (2001) Breakdown of Will. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  3. Ainslie G (2007) Foresight has to pay off in the present moment. Behav Brain Sci 30:313–314Google Scholar
  4. Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Watkins GL, Ponto LL, Hichwa RD (1995) Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. Am J Psychiatry 152:1576–1585PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Atance CM, Meltzoff AN (2007) How developmental science contributes to theories of future thinking. Behav Brain Sci 30:314–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Atance CM, O’Neill DK (2001) Episdodic future thinking. Trends Cognit Sci 5:533–539CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Atance CM, O’Neill DK (2005) The emergence of episodic future thinking in humans. Learn Motiv 36:126–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beran MJ, Evans TA (2006) Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behav Process 73:315–324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Beran MJ, Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Pate JL, Rumbaugh DM (1999) Delay of gratification in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Dev Psychobiol 34:119–127PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Boesch C, Boesch-Acherman H (2000) The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  11. Bolles RC (1970) Species-specific defence reactions and avoidance learning. Psych Rev 77:32–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cognit Sci 11:49–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Byrne R (1995) The thinking ape: evolutionary origins of intelligence. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  14. Call J (2006) Descartes’ two errors: reason and reflection in the great apes. In: Hurley S, Nudds M (eds) Rational animals. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 219–234Google Scholar
  15. Carlson SM, Moses LJ (2001) Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Dev 72:1032–1053PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Christoff K, Ream JM, Gabrieli JDE (2004) Neural basis of spontaneous thought processes. Cortex 40:623–630PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1998) Episodic-like memory during cache recovery by scrub jays. Nature 395:272–274PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Clayton NS, Dickinson A (1999) Scrub jays (Aphelocoma coerulescens) remember the relative time of cahing as well as the location and content of their caches. J Comp Psychol 113:403–416PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Correia SPC, Dickinson A, Clayton NS (2007) Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state. Curr Biol 17:856–861PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. de Waal FBM (1982) Chimpanzee politics: power and sex among apes. Jonathan Cape, LondonGoogle Scholar
  21. Duckworth AL, Seligman MEP (2005) Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychol Sci 16:939–944PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Emery NJ (2006) Cognitive ornithology: the evolution of avian intelligence. Philos Trans R Soc B 361:23–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Evans TA, Beran MJ (2007) Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity. Biol Lett, doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2007.0399
  24. Forzano LB, Logue AW (1992) Predictors of adult humans’ self-control and impulsiveness for food reinforcers. Appetite 19:33–47PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fredrick S, Loewenstein G, O’Donoghue T (2002) Time discounting: a critical review. J Econ Lit 40:351–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gardiner JM (2002) Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a first person approach. In: Baddeley A, Conway M, Aggleton J (eds) Episodic memory–new directions in research. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 11–30Google Scholar
  27. Gilbert DT, Wilson TD (2007) Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science 317:1351–1354PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Gulz A (1991) The planning of action as a cognitive and biological phenomenon. Lund Univ Cognit Stud 2:1–187Google Scholar
  29. Ingvar DH (1979) “Hyperfrontal” distribution of the cerebral grey matter flow in resting wakefulness: on the functional anatomy of the conscious state. Acta Neurol Scand 60:12–25PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Köhler W (1921) Zur psychologie des schimpansen. Psychol Forsch 1:2–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Köhler W (1925) The mentality of apes. Penguin Books, LondonGoogle Scholar
  32. Maslow AH (1943) A theory of human motivation. Psych Rev 50:370–396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Macphail EM (1998) The evolution of consciousness. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  34. Mischel W, Shoda Y, Rodriguez M (1989) Delay of gratification in children. Science 244:933–938PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Mulcahy NJ, Call J (2006) Apes save tool for future use. Science 312:1038–1040PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mulcahy NJ, Call J, Dunbar R (2005) Gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) encode relevant problem features in a tool-using task. J Comp Psychol 119:23–32PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Naqshbandi M, Roberts WA (2006) Anticipation of future events in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and rats (Rattus norvegicus): Test of the Bischof-Kohler hypothesis. J Comp Psychol 120:345–357PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Noble W, Davidson I (1996) Human evolution, language and mind: a psychological and archaeological inquiry. Cambridge University Press, MelbourneGoogle Scholar
  39. O’Doherty J, Rolls ET, Francis S, Bowtell R, McGlone F, Kobal G, Renner B, Ahne G (2000) Senosory-specific satiety-related olfactory activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroreport 11:399–403PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Osvath M, Gärdenfors P (2005) Oldowan culture and the evolution of anticipatory cognition. Lund Univ Cognit Stud 122:1–16Google Scholar
  41. Premack D (2007) Humans and animal cognition: continuity and discontinuity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:13861–13867PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Raby CR, Alexis DM, Dickinson A, Clayton NS (2007) Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature 445:919–921PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Ramseyer A, Pelé M, Dufour V, Chauvin C, Thierry B (2006) Accepting loss: the temporal limits of reciprocity in brown capuchin monkeys. Proc R Soc B 273:179–184PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rilling JK, Barks SK, Parr LA, Preuss TM, Faber TL, Pagnoni G, Bremner JD, Votaw JR (2007) A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:17146–17151PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Roberts WA (2002) Are animals stuck in time? Psych Bull 128:473–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Roberts WA (2006) The questions of temporal and spatial displacement in animal cognition. In: Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (eds) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 145–63Google Scholar
  47. Rosati AG, Stevens JR, Hare B, Hauser MD (2007) The evolutionary origins of human patience: temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Curr Biol 17:1663–1668PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schwartz BL, Hoffman ML, Evans S (2005) Episodic-like memory in a gorilla: a review and new findings. Learn Motiv 36:226–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Seligman MEP (1970) On the generality of the laws of learning. Psych Rev 77:406–418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Shettleworth SJ (2007) Planning for breakfast. Nature 445:825–826PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Small DM, Zatorre RJ, Dagher A, Evans AC, Jones-Gotman M (2001) Changes in brain activity related to eating chocolate: from pleasure to aversion. Brain 124:1720–1733PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Stevens J, Rosati A, Ross K, Hauser M (2005) Will travel for food: spatial discounting in two new world monkeys. Curr Biol 15:1855–1860PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Suddendorf T (2006) Foresight and evolution of the human mind. Science 312:1006–1007PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Suddendorf T, Busby J (2005) Making decisions with the future in mind. Learn Motiv 36:110–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (1997) Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genet Soc Gen Psychol Monogr 123:133–167PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30:299–351PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Suddendorf T, Corballis MC (2008). New evidence for animal foresight? Anim Behav. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.006
  58. Szpunar KK, Watson JM, McDermott KB (2007) Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:642–647PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tangney JP, Baumeister RF, Boone AL (2004) High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better grades and interpersonal success. J Pers 72:271–324PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Tobin H, Logue A (1994) Self-control across species. J Comp Psychol 108:126–133PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W (eds) Organization of memory. New York Academic Press, New York, pp 381–403Google Scholar
  62. Tulving E (2005) Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human? In: Terrace H, Metcalfe J (eds) The missing link in cognition: evolution of self-knowing consciousness. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 3–56Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Lunds University Cognitive ScienceLundSweden

Personalised recommendations