Animal Cognition

, Volume 11, Issue 4, pp 587–597 | Cite as

Inferential reasoning by exclusion in pigeons, dogs, and humans

  • Ulrike Aust
  • Friederike Range
  • Michael Steurer
  • Ludwig Huber
Original Paper

Abstract

The ability to reason by exclusion (which is defined as the selection of the correct alternative by logically excluding other potential alternatives; Call in Anim Cogn 9:393–403 2006) is well established in humans. Several studies have found it to be present in some nonhuman species as well, whereas it seems to be somewhat limited or even absent in others. As inconsistent methodology might have contributed to the revealed inter-species differences, we examined reasoning by exclusion in pigeons (n = 6), dogs (n = 6), students (n = 6), and children (n = 8) under almost equal experimental conditions. After being trained in a computer-controlled two-choice procedure to discriminate between four positive (S+) and four negative (S−) photographs, the subjects were tested with displays consisting of one S− and one of four novel stimuli (S′). One pigeon, half of the dogs and almost all humans preferred S′ over S−, thereby choosing either by novelty, or by avoiding S− without acquiring any knowledge about S′, or by inferring positive class membership of S′ by excluding S−. To decide among these strategies the subjects that showed a preference for S′ were then tested with displays consisting of one of the S′ and one of four novel stimuli (S′′). Although the pigeon preferentially chose the S′′ and by novelty, dogs and humans maintained their preference for S′, thereby showing evidence of reasoning by exclusion. Taken together, the results of the present study suggest that none of the pigeons, but half of the dogs and almost all humans inferred positive class membership of S′ by logically excluding S−.

Keywords

Reasoning by exclusion Categorization Pigeons Dogs Humans 

Supplementary material

10071_2008_149_MOESM1_ESM.doc (26 kb)
S1 (DOC 26 kb)

S2 (MOV 3598 kb)

S3 (MOV 3743 kb)

S4 (MOV 3969 kb)

References

  1. Agnetta B, Hare B, Tomasello M (2000) Cues to food locations that domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) of different ages do and do not use. Anim Cogn 3:107–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aguirre G (1978) Retinal degeneration in the dog: rod dysplasia. Exp Eye Res 26:233–253PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aust U, Huber L (2006) Does the use of natural stimuli facilitate amodal completion in pigeons? Perception 35:333–349PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Aust U, Apfalter W, Huber L (2005) Pigeon categorization: classification strategies in a non-linguistic species. In: Grialou IP, Longo G, Okada M (eds) Images and reasoning. interdisciplinary conference series on reasoning studies. Keio University Press, Tokyo, pp 183–204Google Scholar
  5. Behrend DA, Scofield J, Kleinknecht EE (2001) Beyond fast mapping: young children’s extensions of novel words and novel facts. Dev Psychol 37:698–705PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beran MJ, Washburn DA (2002) Chimpanzee responding during conditional matching-to-sample: control by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 78:497–508PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Call J (2006) Inferences by exclusion in the great apes: the effect of age and species. Anim Cogn 9:393–403PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cerutti DT, Rumbaugh DM (1993) Stimulus relations in comparative primate perspective. Psychol Rec 43:811–821Google Scholar
  9. Clement TS, Zentall TR (2000) Stimulus relations in comparative primate perspective. Psychol Sci 11:261–264PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Clement TS, Zentall TR (2003) Choice based on exclusion in pigeons. Psychon Bull Rev 10:959–964PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Coile DC, Pollitz CH, Smith JC (1989) Behavioral determination of critical flicker fusion in dogs. Physiol Behav 45:1087–1092PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cumming WW, Berryman R (1961) Some data on matching behavior in the pigeon. J Exp Anal Behav 4:281–284PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. D’Amato MR, Colombo M (1989) Serial learning with wild card items by monkeys (Cebus apella): implications for knowledge of ordinal position. J Comp Psychol 103:252–261PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Delius JD, Jitsumori M, Siemann M (2000) Stimulus equivalencies through discrimination reversals. In: Heyes C, Huber L (eds) The evolution of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 103–122Google Scholar
  15. Dixon LS (1977) The nature of control by spoken words over visual stimulus selection. J Exp Anal Behav 27:433–442PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Erdöhegyi A, Topal J, Virányi Z, Miklosi à (2007) Dog-logic: inferential reasoning in a two-way choice task and its restricted use. Anim Behav 74:725–737CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ferrari C, de Rose JC, McIlvane WJ (1993) Exclusion vs. selection training of auditory-visual conditional relations. J Exp Child Psychol 56:49–63PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Halberda J (2003) The development of a word-learning strategy. Cognition 87:B23–B34PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hashiya K, Kojima S (2001) Hearing and auditory–visual intermodal recognition in the chimpanzee. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo, pp 155–189Google Scholar
  20. Herman LM, Richards DG, Wolz JP (1984) Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins. Cognition 16:129–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Horne PJ, Lowe CF (1996) On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. J Exp Anal Behav 65:185–241PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Huber L (2001) Visual categorization in pigeons. In: Cook RG (ed) Avian visual cognition [On-line]Google Scholar
  23. Huber L, Aust U (2006) A modified feature theory as an account to pigeon visual categorization. In: Zentall TR, Wasserman E (eds) Comparative cognition: experimental explorations of animal intelligence. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 325–342Google Scholar
  24. Jaswal VK, Markman EM (2001) Learning proper and common names in inferential versus ostensive contexts. Child Dev 72:768–786PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kaminski J, Call J, Fischer J (2004) Word learning in a domestic dog: evidence for “fast mapping”. Science 304:1682–1683PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kastak CR, Schusterman RJ (2002) Sea lions and equivalence: expanding classes by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 78:449–465PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mackintosh NJ (2000) Abstraction and discrimination. In: Heyes C, Huber L (eds) The evolution of cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 123–141Google Scholar
  28. Macphail EM (1987) The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behav Brain Sci 10:645–695CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Markman EM, Wachtel GF (1988) Children’s use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cogn Psychol 20:121–157CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. McIlvane WJ, Kledaras JB, Munson LC, King KAJ, de Rose JC, Stoddard LT (1987) Controlling relations in conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion. J Exp Anal Behav 48:187–208PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Meehan EF (1995) Emergence by exclusion. Psychol Rec 45:133–154Google Scholar
  32. Mervis CB, Bertrand J (1994) Acquisition of the novel name—nameless category (n3c) principle. Child Dev 65:1646–1662PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Miklosi Ã, Polgardi R, Topal J, Csanyi V (1998) Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs. Anim Cogn 1:113–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Murphy CJ, Zadnik K, Mannis MJ (1992) Myopia and refractive error in dogs. Invest Ophtmol Vis Sci 33:2459–2463Google Scholar
  35. Neitz J, Geist T, Jacobs G (1989) Color vision in the dog. Visual Neurosci 3:119–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Odom JV, Bromberg NM, Dawson WW (1983) Canine visual acuity: retinal and cortical field potentials evoked by pattern stimulation. Am J Physiol 245:R637–R641PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Peichl L (1991) Catecholaminergic amacrine cells in the dog and wolf retina. Vis Neurosci 7:575–587PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Premack D (1995) Cause/induced motion; intention/spontaneous motion. In: Changeux JP, Chavaillon J (eds) Origins of the human brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 286–308Google Scholar
  39. Premack D, Premack AJ (1994) Levels of causal understanding in chimpanzees and children. Cognition 50:347–362PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Range F, Aust U, Steurer M, Huber L (2007) Visual categorization of natural stimuli by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim Cogn. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0123-2
  41. Riedel J, Buttelmann D Call J, Tomasello M (2006) Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use a physical marker to locate hidden food. Anim Cogn 9:27–35PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Savage-Rumbaugh ES (1986) Ape language: from conditioned response to symbol. Columbia University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  43. Schusterman RJ, Krieger K (1984) California sea lions are capable of semantic comprehension. Psychol Rec 34:3–23Google Scholar
  44. Stromer R (1989) Symmetry of control by exclusion in humans’ arbitrary matching to sample. Psychol Rep 64:915–922PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Szetei V, Miklosi A, Topal J, Csanyi V (2003) When dogs seem to lose their nose: an investigation on the use of visual and olfactory cues in communicative context between dog and owner. Appl Anim Behav Sci 83:141–152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Terrace HS, Chen S, Newman AB (1995) Serial learning with a wild card by pigeons (Columba livia): effect of list length. J Comp Psychol 109:162–172PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Tomonaga M (1993) Tests for control by exclusion and negative stimulus relations of arbitrary matching to sample in a “symmetry-emergent” chimpanzee. J Exp Anal Behav 59:215–229PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tomonaga M (1999) Establishing functional equivalence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) with a two-item sequential-responding procedure. J Exp Anal Behav 72:57–79PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2000) Sequential responding to arabic numerals with wild cards by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 3:1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T, Fujita K, Yamamoto J (1991) Emergence of symmetry in a visual conditional discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychol Rep 68:51–60PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Vincent-Smith L, Bricker DD, Bricker W (1974) Acquisition of receptive vocabulary in the toddlerage child. Child Dev 45:189–193PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wilkinson KM, Dube WV, McIlvane WJ (1998) Fast mapping and exclusion (emergent matching) in developmental language behavior analysis and animal cognition research. Psychol Rec 48:407–422Google Scholar
  53. Zentall TR, Edwards CA, Moore BS, Hogan DE (1981) Identity: the basis for both matching and oddity learning. J Exp Psychol: Anim Behav Process 7:70–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ulrike Aust
    • 1
  • Friederike Range
    • 1
  • Michael Steurer
    • 1
  • Ludwig Huber
    • 1
  1. 1.Department for Neurobiology and Cognition ResearchUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria

Personalised recommendations