Animal Cognition

, Volume 9, Issue 4, pp 369–376 | Cite as

Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors

Original Article

Abstract

Like other corvids, food-storing ravens protect their caches from being pilfered by conspecifics by means of aggression and by re-caching. In the wild and in captivity, potential pilferers rarely approach caches until the storers have left the cache vicinity. When storers are experimentally prevented from leaving, pilferers first search at places other than the cache sites. These behaviours raise the possibility that ravens are capable of withholding intentions and providing false information to avoid provoking the storers' aggression for cache protection. Alternatively, birds may refrain from pilfering to avoid conflicts with dominants. Here we examined whether ravens adjust their pilfer tactics according to social context and type of competitors. We allowed birds that had witnessed a conspecific making caches to pilfer those caches either in private, together with the storer, or together with a conspecific bystander that had not created the caches (non-storer) but had seen them being made. Compared to in-private trials, ravens delayed approaching the caches only in the presence of storers. Furthermore, they quickly engaged in searching away from the caches when together with dominant storers but directly approached the caches when together with dominant non-storers. These findings demonstrate that ravens selectively alter their pilfer behaviour with those individuals that are likely to defend the caches (storers) and support the interpretation that they are deceptively manipulating the others' behaviour.

Keywords

Raven Corvus corax Food caching Deception 

References

  1. Anderson JR, Vick S-J, Kuroshima H, Kuwahatan H, Fujita K (2001) Training squirrel monkeys (Saimiri scriureus) to deceive: acquisition and analysis of behaviour toward cooperative and competitive trainers. J Comp Psychol 115:282–293PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Axelrod R, Hamilton WD (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science 211:1390–1396PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barta A, Giraldeau L-A (1998) The effect of dominance hierarchy on the use of alternative foraging tactics: a phenotype-limited producer–scrounger game. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 42:217–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bertram BCR (1978) Living in groups: predators and prey. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioral ecology: an evolutionary approach, 1st edn. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 64–96Google Scholar
  5. Bugnyar T, Heinrich B (2003) Hiding in food caching ravens, Corvus corax. Rev Etol Suppl 5:57Google Scholar
  6. Bugnyar T, Heinrich B (2005) Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgeable and ignorant competitors. Proc R Soc Lond B 272:1641–1646CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K (2002a) Observational learning and the raiding of food caches in ravens, Corvus corax: is it “tactical” deception? Anim Behav 64:185–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K (2002b) Scrounging tactics in free-ranging ravens. Ethology 108:993–1009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bugnyar T, Kotrschal K (2004) Leading a conspecific away from food in ravens, Corvus corax? Anim Cogn 7:69–76PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bugnyar T, Stöwe M, Heinrich B (2004) Ravens, Corvus corax, follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:1331–1336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Byrne RW, Whiten A (1985) Tactical deception of familiar individuals in baboons (Papio ursinus). Anim Behav 33:669–672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Byrne RW, Whiten A (1990) Tactical deception in primates: the 1990 database. Primate Rep 27:1–101Google Scholar
  13. Byrne RW, Whiten A (1992) Cognitive evolution in primates: evidence from tactical deception. Man 27:609–627CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cheney DL, Seyfarth R (1990a) How monkeys see the world: inside the mind of another species. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  15. Cheney DL, Seyfarth R (1990b) Attending to behavior versus attending to knowledge: examining monkeys’ attribution of mental states. Anim Behav 40:742–753CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Coussi-Korbel S (1994) Learning to outwit a competitor in mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus torquatus). J Comp Psychol 108:164–171PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2005a) Cache protection strategies by Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica): implications for social cognition. Anim Behav 70:1251–1263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dally JM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2005b) The social suppression of caching in Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica). Behaviour 142:961–977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dawkins R, Krebs JR (1978) Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioral ecology: an evolutionary approach, 1st edn. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 282–309Google Scholar
  20. Dennett DC (1988) The intentional stance in theory and practise. In: Byrne RW, Whiten A (eds) Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 180–202Google Scholar
  21. Ducoing AM, Thierry B (2003) Withholding information in semifree-ranging tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana). J Comp Psychol 117:67–75PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2001) Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414:443–446PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2004a) The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306:1903–1907PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Emery NJ, Clayton NS (2004b) Comparing the complex cognition of birds and primates. In: Rogers LJ, Kaplan G (eds) Comparative vertebrate cognition: are primates superior to non-primates? Kluwer Academic, Hague, The Netherlands, pp 3–55Google Scholar
  25. Emery NJ, Dally JM, Clayton NS (2004) Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica) use cognitive strategies to protect their caches from thieving conspecifics. Anim Cogn 7:37–43PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fujita K, Kuroshima H, Masuda T (2002) Do tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) spontaneously deceive opponents? A preliminary analysis of an experimental food-competition contest between monkeys. Anim Cogn 5:19–25PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Giraldeau L-A, Caraco T (2000) Social foraging theory. Princeton University Press, Princeton New JerseyGoogle Scholar
  28. Güzeldere G, Nahmias E, Deaner R (2002) Darwin's continuum and the building blocks of deception. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt GM (eds) The cognitive animal. Empirical and theoretical perspectives on animal cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 353—362Google Scholar
  29. Hare B, Call J, Agnetta B, Tomasello M (2000) Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Anim Behav 59:771–785PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hare B, Call J, Tomasello M (2001) Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Anim Behav 61:139–151PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hauser MD (1997) Minding the behaviour of deception. In: Whiten A, Byrne RW (eds) Machiavellian intelligence II: Extensions and evaluations. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 112–143Google Scholar
  32. Heinrich B (1999) Mind of the raven. Harper-Collins, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  33. Heinrich B, Pepper J (1998) Influence of competitors on caching behavior in common ravens, Corvus corax. Anim Behav 56:1083–1090PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Held S, Mendl M, Devereux C, Byrne RW (2000) Social tactics of pigs in a competitive foraging task: the “informed forager paradigm”. Anim Behav 59:569–576PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Held S, Mendl M, Devereux C, Byrne RW (2002) Foraging pigs alter their behavior in response to exploitation. Anim Behav 64:157–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Heyes CM (1998) Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behav Brain Sci 21:101–148PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hirata S, Matsuzawa T (2001) Tactics to obtain a hidden food item in chimpanzee pairs (Pan troglodythes). Anim Cogn 4: 285–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kabicher G, Fritz J (1996) Handaufzucht von Kolkraben. ÖKO-L 18:21–28Google Scholar
  39. Kijne M, Kotrschal K (2002) Neophobia affects choice of food-item size in group foraging common ravens (Corvus corax). Acta Ethol 5:13–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Krebs JR, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioral ecology: an evolutionary approach, 2nd edn. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 380–402Google Scholar
  41. Kummer H, Anzenberger G, Hemelrijk CK (1996) Hiding and perspective taking in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Psychol 110:97–102PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Laland KN (2004) Social learning strategies. Learn Behav 32:4–14PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Maynard Smith J (1979) Game theory and the evolution of behaviour. Proc R Soc Lond B 205:475–488CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Menzel EW Jr (1974) A group of young chimpanzees in a one-acre field: leadership and communication. In: Schrier AM, Stollnitz F (eds) Behavior of nonhuman primates, vol 5. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp 83–153Google Scholar
  45. Mitchell RW (1986) A framework for discussing deception. In: Mitchell RW, Thompson NS (eds) Deception: perspectives on human and non-human deceit. State University of New York Press, Albany, NY, pp 3–10Google Scholar
  46. Mitchell RW, Anderson JR (1997) Pointing, withholding information, and deception in capuchin monkeys. J Comp Psychol 114:351–361CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Mundry R, Fischer J (1998) Use of statistical programs for nonparametric tests of small samples often leads to incorrect P values: examples from Animal Behaviour. Anim Behav 56:256–259PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Povinelli DJ (2001) On the possibilities of detecting intentions prior to understanding them. In: Malle B, Baldwin D, Moses L (eds) Intentionality: a key to human understanding. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 225–248Google Scholar
  49. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 1:515–526Google Scholar
  50. Pulliam HR, Pyke GH, Caraco T (1982) The scanning behavior of juncos: a game-theoretical approach. J Theor Biol 95:89–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ranta E, Peuhkuri N, Laurila A, Rita H, Metcalfe NB (1996) Producers, scroungers and foraging group structure. Anim Behav 51:171–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Semple S, McComb K (1996) Behavioral deception. Tree 11:434—437Google Scholar
  53. Siegel S, Castellan NJ (1988) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioural sciences, 2nd edn. McGraw Hill, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  54. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry, 3rd edn. Freeman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  55. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  56. Watanabe S, Huber L (2006) Animal logics: decision in the absence of human language. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-006-0043-6Google Scholar
  57. Whiten A, Byrne RW (1988a) Tactical deception in primates. Behav Brain Sci 11:233–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Whiten A, Byrne RW (1988b) The manipulation of attention in primate tactical deception. In: Byrne RW, Whiten A (eds) Machiavellian intelligence: social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 211–223Google Scholar
  59. Woodruff G, Premack D (1979) Intentional communication in the chimpanzee: the development of deception. Cognition 7:333–362CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2006

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of BiologyUniversity of VermontBurlingtonUSA
  2. 2.Konrad Lorenz Research Station Gruenau and Department of Behaviour, Neurobiology, CognitionUniversity of ViennaViennaAustria

Personalised recommendations