Animal Cognition

, Volume 8, Issue 4, pp 253–262 | Cite as

Expectations about numerical events in four lemur species (Eulemur fulvus, Eulemur mongoz, Lemur catta and Varecia rubra)

  • Laurie R. Santos
  • Jennifer L. Barnes
  • Neha Mahajan
Original Article

Abstract

Although much is known about how some primates—in particular, monkeys and apes—represent and enumerate different numbers of objects, very little is known about the numerical abilities of prosimian primates. Here, we explore how four lemur species (Eulemur fulvus, E. mongoz, Lemur catta, and Varecia rubra) represent small numbers of objects. Specifically, we presented lemurs with three expectancy violation looking time experiments aimed at exploring their expectations about a simple 1+1 addition event. In these experiments, we presented subjects with displays in which two lemons were sequentially added behind an occluder and then measured subjects’ duration of looking to expected and unexpected outcomes. In experiment 1, subjects looked reliably longer at an unexpected outcome of only one object than at an expected outcome of two objects. Similarly, subjects in experiment 2 looked reliably longer at an unexpected outcome of three objects than at an expected outcome of two objects. In experiment 3, subjects looked reliably longer at an unexpected outcome of one object twice the size of the original than at an expected outcome of two objects of the original size. These results suggest that some prosimian primates understand the outcome of simple arithmetic operations. These results are discussed in light of similar findings in human infants and other adult primates.

Keywords

Prosimians Lemurs Expectancy violation paradigm Number representation 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Courtney Glavis-Bloom, Shannon Kundey, Webb Phillips, and Mariko Yamaguchi for help in pilot work and Valerie Kuhlmeier for her help running these studies. The authors would also like to thank Elizabeth Brannon and Monica Hoffine for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. The authors are also indebted to Allan Wagner for his assistance in securing this field site—the Myakka City Lemur Reserve. We are grateful to the Board of Directors of the Lemur Conservation Foundation and the staff, Penelope Bodry-Sanders, Monica Hoffine, and Kate Chapman. This research was supported by generous summer grants from the Yale College Dean’s Research Fellowship Program to J.B. and N.M and the Ezra Stiles Linck Fellowship to N.M. This work was approved by the Lemur Conservation Foundation IACUC committee and conforms to federal guidelines for the use of animals in research

References

  1. Beran MJ (2004) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) respond to nonvisible sets after one-by-one addition and removal of items. J Comp Psychol 118:25–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beran MJ, Rumbaugh DM (2001) Constructive’ enumeration by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) on a computerized task. Anim Cogn 4:81–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beran MJ, Rumbaugh DM, Savage-Rumbaugh ES (1998) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) counting in a computerized testing paradigm. Psychol Rec 48:3–19Google Scholar
  4. Biro D, Matsuzawa T (2001) Use of numerical symbols by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): cardinals, ordinals, and the introduction of zero. Anim Cogn 4:93–199Google Scholar
  5. Boysen ST, Berntson GG (1989) Numerical competence in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 103:23–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boysen ST Berntson GG, Hannan MB, Cacioppo JT (1996) Quantity-based interference and symbolic representations in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22:76–86CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Boysen ST, Capaldi EJ (1993) The development of numerical competence: animal and human models. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J.Google Scholar
  8. Brannon EM, Terrace HS (1998) Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science 282:746–749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brannon EM, Terrace HS (2000) Representation of the numerosities 1–9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 26:31–49CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Chiang WC, Wynn K (2000) Infants’ representation and tracking of multiple objects. Cognition 77:169–195Google Scholar
  11. Clearfield MW, Mix KS (1999) Number versus contour length in infants’ discrimination of small visual sets. Psychol Sci 1:408–411Google Scholar
  12. Clearfield MW, Mix KS (2001) Amount versus number: infants’ use of area and contour length to discriminate small sets. J Cogn Dev 2:243–260Google Scholar
  13. Feigenson L, Carey S, Hauser MD (2002) The representations underlying infants’ choice of more: object files versus analog magnitudes. Psychol Sci 13:150–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Feigenson L, Carey S, Spelke E (2002) Infants’ discrimination of number vs. continuous extent. Cogn Psychol 44:33–66Google Scholar
  15. Flombaum JI, Junge JA, Hauser MD (2005) Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition (in press)Google Scholar
  16. Gallistel CR (1990) The organization of learning. MIT Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  17. Hauser MD (2000) Wild minds: what animals really think. Holt, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Hauser MD, Carey S (1998) Building a cognitive creature from a set of primitives: evolutionary and developmental insights. In: Allen C, Cummins D (ed) The evolution of mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 51–106Google Scholar
  19. Hauser MD, Carey S (2003) Spontaneous representations of small numbers of objects by rhesus macaques: examinations of content and format. Cogn Psychol 47:367–401Google Scholar
  20. Hauser MD, Carey S, Hauser LB (2000) Spontaneous number representation in semi-free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Proc R Soc Lond Biol Sci 267:829–833Google Scholar
  21. Hauser MD, Dehaene S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Patalano A (2002) Spontaneous number discrimination of multi-format auditory stimuli in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Cognition 86:B23–B32Google Scholar
  22. Hauser MD, MacNeilage P, Ware M (1996) Numerical representations in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci 93:1514–1517CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Hauser MD, Spelke ES (2004) Evolutionary and developmental foundations of human knowledge: a case study of mathematics. In: Gazzaniga M (ed) The cognitive neurosciences III. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 853–864Google Scholar
  24. Hauser MD, Tsao F, Garcia P, Spelke ES (2003) Evolutionary foundations of number: spontaneous representations of numerical magnitudes by cotton-top tamarins. Proc R Soc Lond B 270:1441–1446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Koechlin JD, Dehaene S, Mehler J (1997) Numerical transformations in five-month-old human infants. Math Cogn 3:89–104Google Scholar
  26. Lewis KP, Jaffe S, Brannon E (2005) Analog number representations in mongoose lemurs (Eulemur mongoz): evidence from a search task. Anim Cogn DOI 10.1007/s10071-004-0251-x
  27. Lipton JS, Spelke ES (2003) Origins of number sense: large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychol Sci 14:396–401CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Matsuzawa T (1985) Use of numbers by a chimpanzee. Nature 315:57–59PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Rumbaugh DM, Savage-Rumbaugh S, Hegel MT (1987) Summation in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 13:107–115Google Scholar
  30. Rumbaugh DM, Washburn DA (1993) Counting by chimpanzees and ordinality judgments by macaques in video-formatted tasks. In: Boysen ST, Capaldi JE (ed) The development of numerical competence: animal and human models. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, pp 87–106Google Scholar
  31. Santos LR, Sulkowski G, Spaepen GM, Hauser MD (2002) Object individuation using property/kind information in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Cognition 83:241–264Google Scholar
  32. Simon T, Hespos S, Rochat P (1995) Do infants understand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn (1992). Cogn Dev 10:253–269Google Scholar
  33. Spelke ES (2000) Core knowledge. Am Psychol 55:1233–1243Google Scholar
  34. Tomasello M, Call J (1997) Primate cognition. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Uller C, Hauser M, Carey S (2001) Spontaneous representation of number in cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J Comp Psychol 115:248–257CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Washburn DA, Rumbaugh DM (1991) Ordinal judgments of numerical symbols by macaques (Macaca mulatta). Psychol Sci 2:190–193Google Scholar
  37. West RE, Young RJ (2002) Do domestic dogs show any evidence of being able to count? Anim Cogn 5:183–186PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Wynn K (1992) Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature 358:749–750Google Scholar
  39. Wynn K, Bloom P, Chiang WC (2002) Enumeration of collective entities by 5-month-old infants. Cognition 83:B55–B62Google Scholar
  40. Xu F (2003) Numerosity discrimination in infants: evidence for two systems of representations. Cognition 89:B15–B25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Xu F, Spelke ES (2000) Large number discrimination in 6-month olds. Cognition 74:B1–B11CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Laurie R. Santos
    • 1
  • Jennifer L. Barnes
    • 1
  • Neha Mahajan
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyYale UniversityNew HavenUSA

Personalised recommendations