Advertisement

Animal Cognition

, Volume 7, Issue 4, pp 247–254 | Cite as

One-encounter search-image formation by araneophagic spiders

  • Robert R. Jackson
  • Daiqin Li
Original Article

Abstract

An experimental study of search-image use by araneophagic jumping spiders (i.e., salticid spiders that prey routinely on other spiders) supports five conclusions. First, araneophagic salticids have an innate predisposition to form search images for specific prey from their preferred prey category (spiders) rather than for prey from a non-preferred category (insects). Second, single encounters are sufficient for forming search images. Third, search images are based on selective attention specifically to optical cues. Fourth, there are trade-offs in attention during search-image use (i.e., forming a search image for one type of spider diminishes the araneophagic salticid’s attention to other spiders). Fifth, the araneophagic salticid’s adoption of search images is costly to the prey (i.e., when the araneophagic salticid adopts a search, the prey’s prospects for surviving encounters with the araneophagic salticid are diminished). Cognitive and ecological implications of search-image use are discussed.

Keywords

Search image Selective attention Spiders Portia Scytodes 

Notes

Acknowledgements

Work in the Philippines was generously supported by the International Rice Research Institute (IRRI), and we are especially grateful to Alberto T. Barrion, Kong Luen Heong, and Tom W. Mew for the numerous ways in which they supported the research and to the following IRRI staff for their assistance: Elpie Hernandez, Errol Rico, Ruben Abuyo, Glicerio Javier Jr, Josie Lynn Catindig and Clod Lapis. Our research was funded in part by grants from the Marsden Fund of the New Zealand Royal Society (UOC305) and the National University of Singapore ARC (R-154-000-140-112 and R-154-000-188-112). All work complied with the current laws of New Zealand, the Philippines and Singapore.

References

  1. Blest AD, O’Carroll DC, Carter M (1990) Comparative ultrastructure of Layer I receptor mosaics in principal eyes of jumping spiders: the evolution of regular arrays of light guides. Cell Tissue Res 262:125–141PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Blough PM (1991) Selective attention and search images in pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 17:292–298PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bond AB (1983) Visual search and selection of natural prey stimuli in the pigeons. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 9:292–306PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Bond AB, Kamil AC (1998) Apostatic selection by blue jays produces balanced polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature 395:594–596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bond AB, Kamil AC (2002) Visual predators select for crypticity and polymorphism in virtual prey. Nature 415:609–613CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Broadbent DE (1958) Perception and communication. Pergamon, LondonGoogle Scholar
  7. Carducci JP, Jakob EM (2000) Rearing environment affects behaviour of jumping spiders. Anim Behav 59:39–46CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulmann GL, Petersen SE (1990) Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color and velocity in humans. Science 248:1556–1559PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Croze H (1970) Searching image in carrion crows. Z Tierpsychol 5:1–86Google Scholar
  10. Dabelow S (1958) Zur Biologie der Leimschleuderspinne Scytodes thoracica (Latreille). Zool Jahrb Syst 86:85–126Google Scholar
  11. Dawkins M (1971a) Perceptual changes in chicks: another look at the “search image” concept. Anim Behav 19:566–574Google Scholar
  12. Dawkins M (1971b) Shifts of “attention” in chicks during feeding. Anim Behav 19:575–582Google Scholar
  13. Desimone R, Duncan J (1995) Neural mechanisms of selective attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 18:193–222PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dukas R, Kamil AC (2000) The cost of limited attention in blue jays. Behav Ecol 11:502–506CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dukas R, Kamil AC (2001) Limited attention: the constraint underlying search image. Behav Ecol 12:192–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Foelix RF (1996) Biology of spiders. Oxford University Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  17. Gendron RP (1986) Searching for cryptic prey: evidence for optimal search rates and the formation of search images in quail. Anim Behav 34:898–912Google Scholar
  18. Gendron RP, Staddon JER (1983) Searching for cryptic prey: the effect of search rate. Am Nat 121:172–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gilbert C, Rayor LS (1985) Predatory behavior of spitting spiders (Araneae: Scytodidae) and the evolution of prey wrapping. J Arachnol 13:231–241Google Scholar
  20. Guilford T, Dawkins MS (1987) Search image not proven: appraisal of recent evidence. Anim Behav 35:1838–1845Google Scholar
  21. Grunbaum AA (1927) Uber das Verhalten der Spinne Epeira diademata, besonders gegenuber vibratorischen Reizen. Psychol Forsch 9:275–294Google Scholar
  22. Harland DP, Jackson RR, Macnab AM (1999) Distances at which jumping spiders distinguish between prey and conspecific rivals. J Zool (Lond) 247:357–364Google Scholar
  23. Heijden AHC van der (1992) Selective attention in vision. Chapman and Hall, LondonGoogle Scholar
  24. Hillyard SA, Vogel EK, Luck SJ (1998) Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 353:1257–1270Google Scholar
  25. Homann H (1928) Beiträge zur Physiologie der Spinnenaugen.I. Untersuchungsmethoden, II. Das Sehvermögen der Salticiden. Z Vergl Physiol 7:201–268Google Scholar
  26. Homann H (1971) Die Augen der Araneen. Anatomie, Ontogenie und Bedeutung fur die Systematik (Chelicerate, Arachnida). Z Morphol Oekol Tiere 69:201–272Google Scholar
  27. Jackson RR (1986) Web building, predatory versatility, and the evolution of the Salticidae. In: Shear WA (ed) Spiders: webs, behavior, and evolution. Stanford University Press, Stanford, Calif., pp 232–268Google Scholar
  28. Jackson RR, Carter CM (2001) Interpopulation variation in use of trial-and-error derivation of aggressive-mimicry signals by Portia labiata from the Philippines. J Insect Behav 14:799–827Google Scholar
  29. Jackson RR, Hallas SEA (1986) Comparative biology of Portia africana, P. albimana, P. fimbriata, P. labiata, and P. schultzi, araneophagic web-building jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae): utilisation of silk, predatory versatility, and intraspecific interactions. N Z J Zool 13:423–489Google Scholar
  30. Jackson RR, Pollard SD (1996) Predatory behavior of jumping spiders. Annu Rev Entomol 41:287–308Google Scholar
  31. Jackson RR, Wilcox RS (1994) Spider flexibly chooses aggressive mimicry signals for different prey by trial and error. Behaviour 127:21–36Google Scholar
  32. Jackson RR, Wilcox RS (1998) Spider-eating spiders. Am Sci 86:350–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jackson RR, Li D, Fijn N, Barrion AT (1998) Predator-prey interactions between aggressive-mimic jumping spiders (Salticidae) and araeneophagic spitting spiders (Scytodidae) from the Philippines. J Insect Behav 11:319–342Google Scholar
  34. Jackson RR, Pollard SD, Cerveira CM (2002) Opportunistic use of cognitive smokescreens by araneophagic jumping spiders. Anim Cogn 5:147–157CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kahneman D (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  36. Kastner S, de Weerd P, Desimone R, Ungerleider LG (1998) Mechanisms of directed attention in the human extrastriate cortex as revealed by MRI. Science 282:108–111CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. La Berge D (1995) Attentional processing. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  38. Land MF (1969a) Structure of retinae of the principal eyes of jumping spiders (Salticidae: Dendryphantinae) in relation to visual optics. J Exp Biol 51:443–470PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Land MF (1969b) Movements of retinae of jumping spiders (Salticidae) in response to visual stimuli. J Exp Biol 51:471–493PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Land MF, Nilsson DE (2002) Animal eyes. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  41. Langley CM, Riley DA, Bond AB, Goal N (1995) Visual search for natural grains in pigeons (Columba livia): search images and selective attention. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 22:139–151Google Scholar
  42. Lawrence ES (1986) Can great tits acquire (Parus major) search images? Oikos 47:3–12Google Scholar
  43. Lawrence ES, Allen JA (1983) On the term ‘search image’. Oikos 40:313–314Google Scholar
  44. Li D, Jackson RR (1996) Prey preferences of Portia fimbriata, an araneophagic, web-building jumping spider (Araneae: Salticidae) from Queensland. J Insect Behav 9:613–642Google Scholar
  45. Li D, Jackson RR (1997) Influence of diet on survivorship and growth in Portia fimbriata, an araneophagic jumping spider (Araneae: Salticidae). Can J Zool 75:1652–1658Google Scholar
  46. Li D, Jackson RR (2003) A predator’s preference for egg-carrying prey: a novel cost of parental care. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:129–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Li D, Jackson RR, Barrion AT (1997) Prey preferences of Portia labiata, P. africana, and P. schultzi, araneophagic jumping spiders (Araneae: Salticidae) from the Philippines, Sri Lanka, Kenya and Uganda. N Z J Zool 24:333–349Google Scholar
  48. Li D, Jackson RR, Barrion AT (1999) Parental and predatory behaviour of Scytodes sp., an araneophagic spitting spider (Araneae: Scytodidae) from the Philippines. J Zool (Lond) 247:293–310Google Scholar
  49. McAlister W (1960) The spitting habit in the spider Scytodes intricata Banks (Family Scytodidae). Texas J Sci 12:17–20Google Scholar
  50. Maunsell JHR (1995) The brain’s visual world: representation of visual targets in cerebral cortex. Science 270:764–769PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Melcer T, Chiszar D (1989) Striking prey creates a specific chemical search image in rattlesnakes. Anim Behav 37:477–486Google Scholar
  52. Mook JH, Mook LJ, Heikens HS (1960) Further evidence for the role of “searching images” in the hunting behaviour of titmice. Arch Neerland Zool 13:448–465Google Scholar
  53. Morgan RA, Brown JS (1996) Using giving-up densities to detect search images. Am Nat 148:1059–1074CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Morse DH (2000) Flower choice by naive young crab spiders and the effect of subsequent experience. Anim Behav 59:943–951CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Nams VO (1997) Density-dependent predation by skunks using olfactory search images. Oecologia 110:440–448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Navon D, Gopher D (1979) On the economy of the human processing system. Psychol Rev 86:214–255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Nentwig W (1985) Feeding ecology of the tropical spitting spider Scytodes longipes (Araneae, Scytodidae). Oecologia 65:284–288Google Scholar
  58. Pashler (1998) The psychology of attention. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  59. Persons MH, Walker SE, Rypstra AL, Marshall SD (2001) Wolf spider predator avoidance tactics and survival in the presence of diet-mediated predator cues (Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim Behav 61:43–51CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Persons MH, Walker SE, Rypstra AL, Marshall SD (2002) Fitness costs and benefits of antipredator behavior mediated by chemotactile cues in the wolf spider Pardosa milvina (Araneae: Lycosidae). Behav Ecol 13:386–392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Pietrewicz PT, Kamil AC (1979) Search image formation in the blue jay (Cyanocitta cristata). Science 204:1332–1333Google Scholar
  62. Plaisted KC, MacIntosh NJ (1995) Visual search for cryptic stimuli in pigeons: implications for the search image and search rate hypotheses. Anim Behav 50:1219–1232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Posner MI, Petersen SE (1990) The attentional system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:25–42PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Punzo F (1998) Learning and localization of brain function in the tarantula spider, Phonopelma chalcodes (Orthognatha, Theraphosidae). Comp Biochem Physiol 89A:465–470Google Scholar
  65. Rausher MD (1978) Search image for leaf shape in a butterfly. Science 200:1071–1073Google Scholar
  66. Royama T (1970) Factors governing the hunting behavior and selection of food by the great tit (Parus major L.). J Anim Ecol 39:619–668Google Scholar
  67. Schmidt KA (1998) The consequences of partially directed search effort. Evol Ecol 12:263–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Seah WK, Li D (2001) Stabilimenta attract unwelcome predators to orb webs. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:1553–1558CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Shaw ML, Shaw P (1977) Optimal allocation of cognitive resources to spatial locations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 3:201–211CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ (1995) Biometry: the principles of statistics in biological research, 3rd edn. Freeman, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  71. Spitzer H, Desimone R, Moran J (1988) Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. Science 240:338–340PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Tarsitano MS, Jackson RR, Kirchner W (2000) Signals and signal choices made by araneophagic jumping spiders while hunting the orb-weaving spiders Zygiella x-notata and Zosis genicularis. Ethology 106:595–615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Tinbergen L (1960) The natural control of insects in pinewoods 1. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by song birds. Arch Neerland Zool 13:265–343Google Scholar
  74. Wanless FR (1978) A revision of the spider genus Portia (Araneae: Salticidae). Bull Br Mus Nat Hist 34:83–124Google Scholar
  75. Wilcox RS, Jackson RR (1998) Cognitive abilities of araneophagic jumping spiders. In: Pepperberg I, Kamil A, Balda R (ed) Animal cognition in nature. Academic, New York, pp 411–434Google Scholar
  76. Wilcox RS, Jackson RR (2002) Jumping spider tricksters: deceit, predation, and cognition. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt G (eds) The cognitive animal. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 27–33Google Scholar
  77. Williams DS, McIntyre P (1980) The principal eyes of a jumping spider have a telephoto component. Nature 228:578–580Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Biological SciencesNational University of SingaporeSingaporeSingapore
  2. 2.Department of ZoologyUniversity of CanterburyChristchurchNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations