Animal Cognition

, Volume 7, Issue 4, pp 231–238

Dolphin pointing is linked to the attentional behavior of a receiver

  • Mark J. XitcoJr.
  • John D. Gory
  • Stan A. KuczajII
Original Article

Abstract

In 2001, Xitco et al. (Anim Cogn 4:115–123) described spontaneous behaviors in two bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) that resembled pointing and gaze alternation. The dolphins’ spontaneous behavior was influenced by the presence of a potential receiver, and the distance between the dolphin and the receiver. The present study adapted the technique of Call and Tomasello [(1994) J Comp Psychol 108:307–317], used with orangutans to test the effect of the receiver’s orientation on pointing in these same dolphins. The dolphins directed more points and monitoring behavior at receivers whose orientation was consistent with attending to the dolphins. The results demonstrated that the dolphins’ pointing and monitoring behavior, like that of apes and infants, was linked to the attentional behavior of the receiver.

Keywords

Dolphin Pointing Attentional behavior Cognition 

References

  1. Au WWL (1993) The sonar of dolphins. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Bates E (1976) Language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. Academic, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  3. Bates E (1979) Intentions, conventions, and symbols. In: Bates E (ed) The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. Academic, New York, pp 33–68Google Scholar
  4. Bruner JS (1975) From communication to language--a psychological perspective. Cognition 3:255–287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bruner JS (1977) Early social interaction and language acquisition. In: Schaffer HR (ed) Studies in mother-infant interaction. Academic, London, pp 271–289Google Scholar
  6. Butterworth G (1998) What is special about pointing? In: Simion F, Butterworth G (eds) The development of sensory, motor, and cognitive abilities in early infancy. Psychology Press, London, pp 169–188Google Scholar
  7. Call J, Tomasello M (1994) Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). J Comp Psychol 108:307–317CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Clark RA (1978) The transition from action to gesture. In: Lock A (ed) Action, gesture, and symbol: the emergence of language. Academic, London, pp 231–257Google Scholar
  9. Corkum V, Moore C (1995) Development of joint visual attention in infants. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ (eds) Joint attention: its origin and role in development. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 61–83Google Scholar
  10. Dawson WW, Carder DP, Ridgway SH, Schmeisser ET (1981) Synchrony of dolphin eye movements and their power density spectra. Comp Biochem Physiol 68:443–449CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Desrochers S, Morissette P, Ricard M (1995) Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ (eds) Joint attention: its origin and role in development. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 85–101Google Scholar
  12. Dobrich W, Scarborough HS (1984) Form and function in early communication: language and pointing gestures. J Exp Child Psychol 38:475–490PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Emery N, Lorincz E, Perrett D, Oram M, Baker C (1997) Gaze following and joint attention in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). J Compe Psychol 111:286–293CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hare B, Brown M, Williamson C, Tomasello M (2002) The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298:1634–1636CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Herman LM (2002) Vocal, social, and self-imitation by bottlenosed dolphins. In: Nehaniv C, Dautehahn K (eds) Imitation in animals and artifacts. MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 63–108Google Scholar
  16. Itakura S, Tanaka M (1998) Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 112:119–126PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Kuczaj SA II, Hendry JL (2003) Does language help animals think? In: Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S (eds) Language in mind: advances in the study of language and thought. MIT Bradford Books, Cambridge, Mass., pp 237–275Google Scholar
  18. Kummer H, Anzenberger G, Hemelrijk C (1996) Hiding and perspective taking in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Psychol 110:97–102CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Leavens DA, Hopkins WD (1999) The whole-hand point: the structure and function of pointing from a comparative perspective. J Comp Psychol 113:417–425CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Mangold R, Pobel R (1988) Informativeness and instrumentality in referential communication. J Lang Soc Psychol 7:181–191Google Scholar
  21. Masur EF (1983) Gestural development, dual-directional signaling, and the transition to words. J Psycholinguist Res 12:93–109Google Scholar
  22. Miklosi A, Polgardi R, Topal J, Csanyi V (2000) Intentional behaviour in dog-human communication: an experimental analysis of “showing” behaviour in the dog. Anim Cogn 3:159–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Miklosi A, Kubinyi E, Topal J, Gacsi M, Viran Z, Csanyi V (2003) A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr Biol 13:763--766CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Miles HL (1990) The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In: Parker ST, Gibson KR (eds) “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 511–539Google Scholar
  25. Moore C, Corkum V (1994) Social understanding at the end of the first year of life. Dev Rev 14:349–372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Nachtigall PE (1986) Vision, audition, and chemoreception in dolphins and other marine mammals. In: Schusterman RJ, Thomas JA, Wood FG (eds) Dolphin cognition and behavior: a comparative approach. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 79–113Google Scholar
  27. Pechmann T, Deutsch W (1982) The development of verbal and nonverbal devices for reference. J Exp Child Psychol 34:330–341Google Scholar
  28. Povinelli D, Eddy T (1996a) What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev Ser 247, 61(3)Google Scholar
  29. Povinelli D, Eddy T (1996b) Factors influencing young chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) recognition of attention. J Comp Psychol 110:336--345PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Reaux JE, Theall LA, Povinelli DJ (1999) A longitudinal investigation of chimpanzees’ understanding of visual perception. Child Dev 70:275–290Google Scholar
  31. Reiss D, Marino L (2001) Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence. Proc Natl Acad Sci 98:5937CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Schaffer HR (1984) The child’s entry into a social world. Academic, LondonGoogle Scholar
  33. Scheumann M, Call J (2004) The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus). Anim Cogn 7: (in press)Google Scholar
  34. Soproni K, Miklosi A, Topal J, Csanyi V (2001) Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs. J Comp Psychol 115:122–126PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Soproni K, Miklosi A, Topal J, Csanyi V (2002) Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures. J Comp Psychol 116:27–34CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Tomasello M (1995) Joint attention as social cognition. In: Moore C, Dunham PJ (eds) Joint attention: its origin and role in development. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 103–130Google Scholar
  37. Tomasello M, Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Kruger A (1993) Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Dev 64:1688–1705PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Tomasello M, Call J, Hare B (1998) Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Anim Behav 55:1063–1069PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Tschudin A, Call J, Dunbar RIM, Harris G, van der Elst C (2001) Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). J Comp Psychol 115:100–105CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Wellman HM (1993) Early understanding of mind: the normal case. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ (eds) Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 10--39Google Scholar
  41. Werner H, Kaplan B (1963) Symbol formation. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  42. Xitco MJ Jr (1988) Mimicry of modeled behaviors by bottlenose dolphins. Master’s thesis, University of Hawaii, HonoluluGoogle Scholar
  43. Xitco MJ Jr, Gory JD, Kuczaj SA II (2001) Spontaneous pointing by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Anim Cogn 4:115–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Zinober B, Martlew M (1985) The development of communicative gestures. In: Barrett M (ed) Children’s single-word speech. Wiley, London, pp 183–215Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mark J. XitcoJr.
    • 1
  • John D. Gory
    • 2
  • Stan A. KuczajII
    • 3
  1. 1.Space and Naval Warfare Systems CenterSan DiegoUSA
  2. 2.Southern Methodist UniversityDallasUSA
  3. 3.University of Southern MississippiHattiesburgUSA

Personalised recommendations