Animal Cognition

, Volume 6, Issue 4, pp 199–211 | Cite as

The Ai project: historical and ecological contexts

Review

Abstract

This paper aims to review a long-term research project exploring the chimpanzee mind within historical and ecological contexts. The Ai project began in 1978 and was directly inspired by preceding ape-language studies conducted in Western countries. However, in contrast with the latter, it has focused on the perceptual and cognitive capabilities of chimpanzees rather than communicative skills between humans and chimpanzees. In the original setting, a single chimpanzee faced a computer-controlled apparatus and performed various kinds of matching-to-sample discrimination tasks. Questions regarding the chimpanzee mind can be traced back to Wolfgang Koehler’s work in the early part of the 20th century. Yet, Japan has its unique natural and cultural background: it is home to an indigenous primate species, the Japanese snow monkey. This fact has contributed to the emergence of two previous projects in the wild led by the late Kinji Imanishi and his students. First, the Koshima monkey project began in 1948 and became famous for its discovery of the cultural propagation of sweet-potato washing behavior. Second, pioneering work in Africa, starting in 1958, aimed to study great apes in their natural habitat. Thanks to the influence of these intellectual ancestors, the present author also undertook the field study of chimpanzees in the wild, focusing on tool manufacture and use. This work has demonstrated the importance of social and ecological perspectives even for the study of the mind. Combining experimental approaches with a field setting, the Ai project continues to explore cognition and behavior in chimpanzees, while its focus has shifted from the study of a single subject toward that of the community as a whole.

References

  1. Asano T (1976) Some effects of a discrete trial procedure on differentiation learning by Japanese monkeys. Primates 17:53–62Google Scholar
  2. Asano T, Kumazaki K (1975) Self-initiated regulation of the room light by a chimpanzee (in Japanese). Annu Anim Psychol 25:35–42Google Scholar
  3. Asano T, Kojima T, Matsuzawa T, Kubota K, Murofushi K (1982) Object and color naming in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Proc Jpn Acad 58:118–122Google Scholar
  4. Bard K, Platzman K, Lester B, Suomi S (1992) Orientation to social and nonsocial stimuli in neonatal chimpanzees and humans. Infant Behav Dev 15:43–56Google Scholar
  5. Biro D, Matsuzawa T (1999) Numerical ordering in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): planning, executing, monitoring. J Comp Psychol 111:159–173Google Scholar
  6. Biro D, Matsuzawa T (2001a) Chimpanzee numerical competence: cardinal and ordinal skills. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 195–225Google Scholar
  7. Biro D, Matsuzawa T (2001b) Use of numerical symbols by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): cardinals, ordinals, and the introduction of zero. Anim Cogn 4:193–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Biro D, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R, Yamakoshi G, Sousa C, Matsuzawa T (2003) Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: evidence from field experiments. Anim Cogn 6 (in press). DOI 10.1007/s10071–003–0183-xGoogle Scholar
  9. Blough D (1961) Animal psychopysics. Sci Am 205:113–122Google Scholar
  10. Boesch C , Boesch-Achermann H (2000) The chimpanzees of the Tai Forest: behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  11. Fagot J, Tomonaga M (1999) Global and local processing in humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): use of a visual search task with compound stimuli. J Comp Psychol 113:3–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fujita K (1982) An analysis of stimulus control in two-color matching-to-sample behaviors of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata fuscata). Jpn Psychol Res 24:124–135Google Scholar
  13. Fujita K (1997) Perception of the Ponzo illusion by rhesus monkeys, chimpanzees, and humans: similarity and difference in the three primate species. Percept Psychophys 59:284–292PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Fujita K, Matsuzawa T (1986) A new procedure to study the perceptual world of animals with sensory reinforcement: recognition of humans by a chimpanzee. Primates 27:283–291Google Scholar
  15. Fujita K, Matsuzawa T (1990) Delayed figure reconstruction by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol 104:345–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gardner RA, Gardner BT (1969) Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science 165:664–672PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  18. Hamada Y, Chatani K, Udono T, Kikuchi Y, Gunji H (2003) A longitudinal study on hand and wrist skeletal maturation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), with emphasis on growth in linear dimensions. Primates 44:259–271PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Hashiya K, Kojima T (1997) Auditory-visual intermodal matching by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Jpn Psychol Res 39:182–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hashiya K, Kojima T (2001) In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo, pp 487–508Google Scholar
  21. Hayashi M, Matsuzawa T (2003) Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees. Anim Cogn 6 (in press). DOI 10.1007/s10071–003–0185–8Google Scholar
  22. Hayes C (1951) The ape in our house. Harper, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Hirata S, Celli M (2003) Role of mothers in the acquisition of tool use behaviours by captive infant chimpanzees. Anim Cogn 6 (in press). DOI 10.1007/s10071–003–0187–6Google Scholar
  24. Hirata S, Matsuzawa T (2001) Tactics to obtain a hidden food item in chimpanzee pairs (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 4:285–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hirata S, Morimura N (2000) Naïve chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) observation of experienced conspecifics in tool-using task. J Comp Psychol 114:291–296CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Hirata S, Myowa M, Matsuzawa T (1999) Use of leaves as cushions to sit on wet ground by wild chimpanzees. Am J Primatol 44:215–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hirata S, Watanabe K, Kawai M (2001a) “Sweet-potato washing” revisited. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 487–508Google Scholar
  28. Hirata S, Yamakoshi G, Fujita S, Ohashi G, Matsuzawa T (2001b) Capturing and toying with Hyraxes (Dendrohyrax dorsalis) by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Bossou, Guinea. Am J Primatol 53:93–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Humle T, Matsuzawa T (2001) Behavioral diversity among wild chimpanzee populations of Bossou and neighboring areas, Guinea and Cote d’Ivoire, West Africa. Folia Primatol 72:57–68CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Humle T, Matsuzawa T (2002) Ant-dipping among the chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, and some comparisons with other sites. Am J Primatol 58:133–148CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Imanishi (2002) A Japanese view of nature: the world of living things by Kinji Imanishi. Routledge Curzon, New York (1st published 1941 in Japanese)Google Scholar
  32. Inoue-Nakamura N, Matsuzawa T (1997) Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 111:159–173PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Itakura S (1992) A chimpanzee with the ability to learn the use of personal pronouns. Psychol Rec 42:157–172Google Scholar
  34. Itakura S (1996) An exploratory study of gaze monitoring in nonhuman primates. Jpn Psychol Res 38:174–180Google Scholar
  35. Itakura S, Matsuzawa T (1993) Acquisition of personal pronouns by a chimpanzee. In: Roitblat H, Herman L, Nachtigall P (eds) Language and communication: comparative perspectives. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., pp 347–363Google Scholar
  36. Itani J (1963) Vocal communication of the wild Japanese monkey. Primates 4:11–66Google Scholar
  37. Iversen I, Matsuzawa T (1996) Visually guided drawing in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Jpn Psychol Res 38:126–135Google Scholar
  38. Iversen I, Matsuzawa T (1997) Model-guided line drawing in the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Jpn Psychol Res 39:154–181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Iversen I, Matsuzawa T (2001) Acquisition of navigation by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in an automated finger maze task. Anim Cogn 4:179–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Iversen I, Matsuzawa T (2003) Development of interception of moving targets by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in an automated task. Anim Cogn 6:169–183. DOI 10.1007/s10071–003–0175-xPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Kawai M (1965) Newly acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima islet. Primates 6:1–30Google Scholar
  42. Kawai N (2001) Ordering and planning in sequential responding to Arabic numerals in a chimpanzee. Psychologia 44:60–69Google Scholar
  43. Kawai N, Matsuzawa T (2000). Numerical memory span in a chimpanzee. Nature 403:39–40PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Kellog W, Kellog L (1967) The ape and the child. Hafner, New York (1st published 1933 by McGraw-Hill, New York)Google Scholar
  45. Kimura T (1987) Development of chimpanzee locomotion on level surfaces. Hum Evol 2:107–119Google Scholar
  46. Kimura T (1991) Voluntary bipedal walking of infant chimpanzees. Hum Evol 6:377–390Google Scholar
  47. Kimura T (1996) Centre of gravity of the body during the ontogeny of chimpanzee bipedal walking. Folia Primatol 66:126–136PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Kimura T, Hamada Y (1996) Growth of wild and laboratory born chimpanzees. Primates 37:237–251Google Scholar
  49. Kobayashi H, Koshima S (1997) Unique morphology of the human eye. Nature 387:767–768Google Scholar
  50. Koehler W (1957) The mentality of apes. Penguin, MiddlesexGoogle Scholar
  51. Kojima S (1980) Short-term memory in the rhesus monkeys: a behavioral analysis of delayed-response performance. J Exp Anal Behav 33:359–368Google Scholar
  52. Kojima S (1990) Comparison of auditory functions in the chimpanzee and human. Folia Primatol 55:62–72PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Kojima S (1992) Hearing and speech perception in the chimpanzee. In: Nishida T, McGrew W, Marler P, Pickford M, Waal F de (eds) Topics in primatology. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp 249–255Google Scholar
  54. Kojima S (2003a) A search for the origins of human speech: auditory and vocal functions of the chimpanzee. Kyoto University Academic Press, KyotoGoogle Scholar
  55. Kojima S (2003b) Experimental analyses of body image in the chimpanzee. Primates 44:231–238PubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Kojima S, Kiritani S (1989) Vocal-auditory functions in the chimpanzee: vowel perception. Int J Primatol 10:199–213Google Scholar
  57. Kojima S, Tatsumi I, Kiritani S, Hirose H (1989) Vocal-auditory functions of the chimpanzee: consonant perception. Hum Evol 4:403–416Google Scholar
  58. Kojima S, Izumi A, Ceugniet M (2003) Identification of vocalizers by pant hoots, pant grunts and screams in a chimpanzee. Primates 44:225–230PubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Kojima T (1984) Generalization between productive use and receptive discrimination of names in an artificial visual language by a chimpanzee. Intl J Primatol 5:161–182Google Scholar
  60. Kortlandt A (1986) The use of stone tools by wild-living chimpanzees and earliest hominids. J Hum Evol 15:77–132Google Scholar
  61. Kortlandt A, Holzaus E (1987) New data on the use of stone tools by chimpanzees in Guinea and Liberia. Primates 28:473–496Google Scholar
  62. Ladygina-Kohts N (2002) Infant chimpanzee and human child. Oxford University Press, New York (1st published 1935 in Russian)Google Scholar
  63. Masataka N, Ishida T, Suzuki J, Matsumura S, Udono S, Sasaoka S (1990) Dominance and immunity in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology 85:147–155Google Scholar
  64. Matsuno T, Kawai N, Matsuzawa T (2003) Color classification by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a matching-to-sample task. Behav Brain Res DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4328(03)00185-2Google Scholar
  65. Matsuzawa T (1985a). Use of numbers in a chimpanzee. Nature 315:57–59PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Matsuzawa T (1985b) Color naming and classification in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J Hum Evol 14:283–291Google Scholar
  67. Matsuzawa T (1990a) Form perception and visual acuity in a chimpanzee. Folia Primatol 55:24–32PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Matsuzawa T (1990b) Spontaneous sorting in human and chimpanzee. In: Parker S, Gibson K (eds) Language and intelligence in monkeys and apes: comparative and developmental perspectives. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 451–468Google Scholar
  69. Matsuzawa T (1994) Field experiments on use of stone tools in the wild. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, Waal FBM de, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 351–370Google Scholar
  70. Matsuzawa T (1996) Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and in captivity: isomorphism of symbol use and tool use. In: McGrew W, Marchant L, Nishida T(eds) Great ape societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 196–209Google Scholar
  71. Matsuzawa T (1999) Communication and tool use in chimpanzees: cultural and social contexts. In: Hauser M, Konishi M (eds) The design of animal communication. The MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 645–671Google Scholar
  72. Matsuzawa T (2001a) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg Google Scholar
  73. Matsuzawa T (2001b) Primate foundations of human intelligence: a view of tool use in nonhuman primates and fossil hominids. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp. 557–574Google Scholar
  74. Matsuzawa T (2002) Chimpanzee Ai and her son Ayumu: an episode of education by master–apprenticeship. In: Bekoff M, Allen C, Burghardt G (eds) The cognitive animal. The MIT Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 189–195Google Scholar
  75. Matsuzawa T (2003a) Koshima monkeys and Bossou chimpanzees: culture in nonhuman primates based on long-term researches. In: Waal F de, Tyack P (eds) Animal social complexity. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass., pp 374–387Google Scholar
  76. Matsuzawa T (2003b) From chimpanzees to the human mind. Science 300:2034–2035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Matsuzawa T, Tomonaga M (2001) For a rise of comparative cognitive science. Anim Cogn 4:133–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Matsuzawa T, Yamakoshi G (1996) Comparison of chimpanzee material culture between Bossou and Nimba, West Africa. In: Russon A, Bard K, Parker S (eds) Reaching into thought. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 211–232Google Scholar
  79. Matsuzawa T, Biro D, Humle T, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R, Yamakoshi G (2001) Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master–apprenticeship. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 3–25Google Scholar
  80. McGrew WC (1992) Chimpanzee material culture. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  81. Morimura N, Matsuzawa T (2001) Memory of movies by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 115:152–158CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Murofushi K (1975) Variable criterion analysis of simple reaction time distributions in normal, chiasm-sectioned and chiasm-callosum-sectioned monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Annu Anim Psychol 25:1–17Google Scholar
  83. Murofushi K (1997). Numerical matching behavior by a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): subitizing and analogue magnitude estimation. Jpn Psychol Res 39:140–153CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Myowa M (1996) Imitation of facial gestures by an infant chimpanzee. Primates 37:207–213Google Scholar
  85. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Matsuzawa T (1999) Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 113:128–136CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Matsuzawa T (2000) Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 114:381–391PubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Tomonaga M, Tanaka M, Matsuzawa T (2003) Preference for human direct gaze in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Cognition DOI: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00071-4Google Scholar
  88. Nishida T (1990) The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. University of Tokyo Press, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  89. Nishimura T, Mikami A, Suzuki J, Matsuzawa T (2003) Descent of the larynx in chimpanzee infant. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:6930–6933CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Ochiai T, Matsuzawa T (1997) Planting trees in an outdoor compound of chimpanzees for an enriched environment. Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Environmental Enrichment. Orlando, Fla., pp 355–364Google Scholar
  91. Okamoto S, Tomonaga M, Ishii K, Kawai N, Tanaka M, Matsuzawa T (2002) An infant chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) follows human gaze. Anim Cogn 5:107–114PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Oshiba N (1997) Memorization of serial items by Japanese monkeys, a chimpanzee, and humans. Jpn Psychol Res 39:236–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Povinelli D, Nelson K, Boysen S (1990) Inferences about guessing and knowing by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 104:203–210CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Premack D (1971) Language in chimpanzee? Science 172:808–822PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Premack D, Woodruff G (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav Brain Sci 4:515–526Google Scholar
  96. Rumbaugh DM (1977) Language learning by a chimpanzee: the Lana Project. Academic, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  97. Rumbaugh DM, Gill TV, Glasersfeld EC von (1973) Reading and sentence completion by a chimpanzee (Pan). Science 182:731–733Google Scholar
  98. Sato A, Kanazawa S, Fujita K (1997) Perception of object unity in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Jpn Psychol Res 39:191–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Savage-Rumbaugh S, McDonald K, Sevcik R, Hopkins W, Rubert E (1986) Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). J Exp Psychol Gen 115:211–235CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Sousa C, Matsuzawa T (2001) The use of tokens as rewards and tools by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 4:213–221CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Sousa C, Okamoto S, Matsuzawa T (2003) Behavioural development in a matching-to-sample task and token use by an infant chimpanzee reared by his mother. Anim Cogn 6 (in press). DOI 10.1007/s10071–003–0186–7Google Scholar
  102. Stebbins W (1970) Animal psychophysics. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  103. Sugiyama Y (1994) Tool use by wild chimpanzees. Nature 367:327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Sugiyama Y, Koman J (1979a) Social structure and dynamics of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20:323–339Google Scholar
  105. Sugiyama Y, Koman J (1979b) Tool-using and making behavior in wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20:513–524Google Scholar
  106. Suzuki S, Matsuzawa T (1997) Choice between two discrimination tasks in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Jpn Psychol Res 39:226–235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Takenaka O, Takasaki S, Kawamoto Y, Arakawa M Takenaka A (1993) Polymorphic microsatellite DNA amplification customized for chimpanzee paternity testing. Primates 34:27–35Google Scholar
  108. Takeshita H (2001) Development of combinatory manipulation in chimpanzee infants (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 4:335–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Takeshita H, Hooff J van (1996) Tool use by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) of the Arnhem Zoo community. Jpn Psychol Res 38:163–173Google Scholar
  110. Takeshita H, Walraven V (1996) A comparative study of the variety and complexity of object manipulation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Primates 37:423–441Google Scholar
  111. Tanaka M (1995) Object sorting in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): classification based on physical identity, complementarity, and familiarity. J Comp Psychol 109:151–161CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Tanaka M (1996) Information integration about object-object relationships in chimpanzees. J Comp Psychol 110:323–335CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Tanaka M (2001) Discrimination and categorization of photographs of natural objects by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 4:201–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Tanaka M (2003) Visual preferences by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) for photos of primates measured by a free choice-order task: implication for influence of social experience. Primates 44:157–165PubMedGoogle Scholar
  115. Tomasello M, Kruger A, Ratner H (1993a) Cultural learning. Behav Brain Sci 16:495–552Google Scholar
  116. Tomasello M, Savage-Rumbaugh S, Kruger A (1993b) Imitative learning of actions on objects by children, chimpanzees, and enculturated chimpanzees. Child Dev 64:1688–1705PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Tomonaga M (1993a) Use of multiple-alternative matching-to-sample in the study of visual search in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 107:75–83CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Tomonaga M (1993b) A search for search asymmetry in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Percept Motor Skills 73:1287–1295Google Scholar
  119. Tomonaga M (1998) Perception of shape from shading in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). Anim Cogn 1:25–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Tomonaga M (1999a) Inversion effect in perception of human faces in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Primates 40:417–438Google Scholar
  121. Tomonaga M (1999b) Visual texture segregation by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Behav Brain Res 99:209–218CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  122. Tomonaga M (1999c) Establishing functional classes in a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) with a two-item sequential-responding procedure. J Exp Anal Behav 72:57–59Google Scholar
  123. Tomonaga M (2001) Visual search for biological motion patterns in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychologia 44:46–59Google Scholar
  124. Tomonaga M (2002) Action-based distractor effects on the manual response times of chimpanzees during discrimination tasks. Cogn Brain Res 13:235–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (1992) Perception of complex geometric figures in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens): analyses of visual similarity on the basis of choice reaction time. J Comp Psychol 106:43–52CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2000) Sequential responding to Arabic numerals with wild cards by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 3:1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (2002) Enumeration of briefly presented items by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens). Anim Learn Behav 30:143–157PubMedGoogle Scholar
  128. Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T, Fujita K, Yamamoto J (1991) Emergence of symmetry in a visual conditional discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychol Rep 68:51–60PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Tomonaga M, Itakura S, Matsuzawa T (1993) Superiority of conspecific faces and reduced inversion effect in face perception by a chimpanzee. Folia Primatol 61:110–114PubMedGoogle Scholar
  130. Tonooka R (2001) Leaf-folding behavior for drinking water by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Bossou, Guinea. Anim Cogn 4:325–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Tonooka R, Matsuzawa T (1993) Hand preferences of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in simple reaching for food. Int J Primatol 16:17–35Google Scholar
  132. Tonooka R, Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T (1997) Acquisition and transmission of tool making and use for drinking juice in a group of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Jpn Psychol Res 39:253–265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Torigoe T (1985) Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of nonhuman primates. Primates 26:182–194Google Scholar
  134. Waal F de (2001) The ape and the Sushi master: cultural reflections by a primatologists. Basic, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  135. Watanabe K (2001) A review of 50 years of research on the Japanese monkeys of Koshima: status and dominance. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 405–417Google Scholar
  136. Watanabe K, Mori A, Kawai M (1992) Characteristic features of the reproduction in Koshima monkeys, Macaca fuscata fuscata: a 34-year summary. Primates 33:1–32Google Scholar
  137. Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew W, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin C, Wrangham R, Boesch C (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–685CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  138. Wrangham R, McGrew W, Waal F de, Heltne P (1994) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  139. Yamakoshi G (1998) Dietary responses to fruit scarcity of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea: possible implications for ecological importance of tool use. Am J Phys Anthropol 106:283–295CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  140. Yamakoshi G (2001) Ecology of tool use in wild chimpanzees: toward reconstruction of early hominid evolution. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 537–556Google Scholar
  141. Yerkes RM, Yerkes A (1929) The great apes. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Section of Language and Intelligence, Primate Research InstituteKyoto UniversityJapan

Personalised recommendations