Animal Cognition

, Volume 6, Issue 4, pp 225–233 | Cite as

Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees

Original Article

Abstract

This study focuses on the development of spontaneous object manipulation in three infant chimpanzees during their first 2 years of life. The three infants were raised by their biological mothers who lived among a group of chimpanzees. A human tester conducted a series of cognitive tests in a triadic situation where mothers collaborated with the researcher during the testing of the infants. Four tasks were presented, taken from normative studies of cognitive development of Japanese infants: inserting objects into corresponding holes in a box, seriating nesting cups, inserting variously shaped objects into corresponding holes in a template, and stacking up wooden blocks. The mothers had already acquired skills to perform these manipulation tasks. The infants were free to observe the mothers' manipulative behavior from immediately after birth. We focused on object–object combinations that were made spontaneously by the infant chimpanzees, without providing food reinforcement for any specific behavior that the infants performed. The three main findings can be summarized as follows. First, there was precocious appearance of object–object combination in infant chimpanzees: the age of onset (8–11 months) was comparable to that in humans (around 10 months old).Second, object–object combinations in chimpanzees remained at a low frequency between 11 and 16 months, then increased dramatically at the age of approximately 1.5 years. At the same time, the accuracy of these object–object combinations also increased. Third, chimpanzee infants showed inserting behavior frequently and from an early age but they did not exhibit stacking behavior during their first 2 years of life, in clear contrast to human data.

Keywords

Cognitive development Infant Chimpanzee Object manipulation Object–object combination 

References

  1. Bayley N (1969) Bayley scales of infant development. The Psychological Corporation, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  2. Fragaszy DM, Adams-Curtis LE (1991) Generative aspects of manipulation in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol 105:387–397CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Goodall J (1964) Tool-using and aimed throwing in a community of free-living chimpanzees. Nature 201:1264–1266Google Scholar
  4. Goodall J (1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe: patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  5. Hirata S, Celli ML (2003) Role of mothers in the acquisition of tool use behaviors by captive infant chimpanzees. Anim Cogn (in press)Google Scholar
  6. Ikuzawa M (2000) Developmental diagnostic tests for children, 2nd edn. (in Japanese). Nakanishiya, KyotoGoogle Scholar
  7. Inoue-Nakamura N, Matsuzawa T (1997) Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 111:159–173PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. King WL, Seegmiller B (1973) Performance of 14- to 22-month-old black, firstborn male infants on two tests of cognitive development: The Bayley Scales and the Infant Psychological Development Scale. Dev Psychol 8:317–326Google Scholar
  9. Kopp CB, Sigman M, Parmelee AH (1974) Longitudinal study of sensorimotor development. Dev Psychol 10:687–695Google Scholar
  10. Matsuzawa T (1994) Field experiments on use of stone tools by chimpanzees in the wild. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Mass., pp 351–370Google Scholar
  11. Matsuzawa T (2001) Primate foundations of human intelligence: a view of tool use in nonhuman primates and fossil hominids. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 3–25Google Scholar
  12. Matsuzawa T (2003) Ai project: a retrospective of 25 years research on chimpanzee intelligence. Anim Cogn (in press)Google Scholar
  13. Matsuzawa T, Biro D, Humle T, Inoue-Nakamura N, Tonooka R, Yamakoshi G (2001) Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 557–574Google Scholar
  14. McGrew WC (1992) Chimpanzee material culture: implications for human evolution. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  15. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Matsuzawa T (1999) Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 113:128–136CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Matsuzawa T (2000) Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 114:381–391Google Scholar
  17. Nishida T (1990) The chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: sexual and life history strategies. University of Tokyo Press, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  18. Ochiai T, Matsuzawa T (1997) Planting trees in an outdoor compound of chimpanzees for an enriched environment. In: Hare VJ, Worley KE (eds) Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Environmental Enrichment. The Shape of Enrichment, Orlando, Fla., pp 355–364Google Scholar
  19. Potì P (1996) Spatial aspects of spontaneous object grouping by young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Int J Primatol 17:101–116Google Scholar
  20. Potì P, Spinozzi G (1994) Early sensorimotor development in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J Comp Psychol 108:93–103CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Schaik CP van, Deaner RO, Merrill MY (1999) The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. J Hum Evol 36:719–741PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Takeshita H (2001) Development of combinatory manipulation in chimpanzee infants (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 4:335–345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Takeshita H, Walraven V (1996) A comparative study of the variety and complexity of object manipulation in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus). Primates 37:423–441Google Scholar
  24. Takeshita H, Tanaka M, Matsuzawa T (1989) Development of postural reactions and object manipulation in primate infants (in Japanese with English summary). Primate Res 5:111–120Google Scholar
  25. Tanaka M, Tanaka S (1982) Developmental diagnosis of human infants: from 6 to 18 months (in Japanese). Ootsuki, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  26. Torigoe T (1985) Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates. Primates 26:182–194Google Scholar
  27. Uzgiris IC, Hunt JM (1975) Assessment in infancy: ordinal scales of psychological development. University of Illinois Press, UrbanaGoogle Scholar
  28. Vauclair J, Bard KA (1983) Development of manipulations with objects in ape and human infants. J Hum Evol 12:631–645Google Scholar
  29. Westergaard GC (1993) Development of combinatorial manipulation in infant baboons (Papio cynocephalus anubis). J Comp Psychol 107:34–38Google Scholar
  30. Westergaard GC, Suomi SJ (1994) Hierarchical complexity of combinatorial manipulation in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Am J Primatol 32:171–176Google Scholar
  31. Whiten A, Goodall J, McGrew WC, Nishida T, Reynolds V, Sugiyama Y, Tutin CEG, Wrangham RW, Boesch C (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–685CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Yamakoshi G. (2001) Ecology of tool use in wild chimpanzees: toward reconstruction of early hominid evolution. In: Matsuzawa T (ed) Primate origins of human cognition and behavior. Springer, Tokyo Berlin Heidelberg, pp 537–556Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Section of Language and Intelligence, Primate Research InstituteKyoto UniversityAichi Japan

Personalised recommendations