Advertisement

Journal of Neural Transmission

, Volume 126, Issue 12, pp 1653–1665 | Cite as

Covariation bias in depression - a predictor of treatment response?

  • Saskia Stonawski
  • Julian Wiemer
  • Catherina Wurst
  • Jannika Reitz
  • Leif Hommers
  • Andreas Menke
  • Katharina Domschke
  • Miriam A. Schiele
  • Paul PauliEmail author
Psychiatry and Preclinical Psychiatric Studies - Original Article
  • 87 Downloads

Abstract

Covariation bias, defined as an overestimation of the relationship between fear-relevant stimuli and aversive consequences, is a well-investigated cognitive bias in anxiety disorders. As patients with affective disorders also show biased information processing, the aim of the present study was to investigate whether depressed patients also display a covariation bias between negative stimuli and aversive consequences. Covariation estimates of 62 inpatients with a current severe depressive episode were assessed at admission (n = 31) or after 6 weeks of treatment (n = 31) and were compared in a between-group design with 31 age- and sex-matched healthy controls. All participants showed a covariation bias for the relationship between negative stimuli and aversive consequences. Moreover, covariation bias at admission was significantly associated with various clinician- and self-reported dimensional measures of treatment response assessed 6 weeks later (Global Assessment of Functioning, Clinical Global Impression Scale, and Beck Depression Inventory), i.e., patients with a stronger bias showed greater impairment after 6 weeks of treatment. Categorical analyses revealed that overall, treatment non-responders—but not responders—were characterized by a covariation bias. The naturalistic study design without standardized pharmacological and psychotherapeutic treatments is a central limitation. We conclude that the covariation bias may constitute a possible marker in the field of emotional information processing in the search for effective predictors of therapy outcome.

Keywords

Covariation bias Affective disorders Severe depressive episode Treatment response 

Notes

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical Research (IZKF), University of Wuerzburg (N-258 to LH), and from the German Research Foundation DFG, SFB-TRR-58, projects B01 to PP and project B05, and DFG project 378414384 to JW.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed written consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Amrhein C, Pauli P, Dengler W, Wiedemann G (2005) Covariation bias and its physiological correlates in panic disorder patients. J Anxiety Disord 19(2):177–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Ataya AF, Adams S, Mullings E, Cooper RM, Attwood AS, Munafo MR (2012) Methodological considerations in cognitive bias research: the next steps. Drug Alcohol Depend 124(3):191–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beck AT (1976) Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. International Universities Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck AT, Bredemeier K (2016) A unified model of depression: Integrating clinical, cognitive, biological, and evolutionary perspectives. Clin Psychol Sci 4(4):596–619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. de Jong PJ, Merckelbach H (1993) Covariation bias, classical conditioning, and phobic fear. Integr Physiol Behav Sci 28(2):167–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. de Jong PJ, Merckelbach H, Arntz A, Nijman H (1992) Covariation detection in treated and untreated spider phobics. J Abnorm Psychol 101(4):724–727CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. de Jong PJ, van den Hout MA, Merckelbach H (1995a) Covariation bias and the return of fear. Behav Res Ther 33(2):211–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. de Jong PJ, Merckelbach H, Arntz A (1995b) Covariation bias in phobic women: the relationship between a priori expectancy, on-line expectancy, autonomic responding, and a posteriori contingency judgment. J Abnorm Psychol 104(1):55–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. De Jong PJ, Merckelbach H, Bogels S, Kindt M (1998) Illusory correlation and social anxiety. Behav Res Ther 36(11):1063–1073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Domschke K, Muller DJ, Serretti A (2015) Personalized therapies in psychiatry: promises, pitfalls and perspectives. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 122(1):1–3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fava M (2003) Diagnosis and definition of treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry 53(8):649–659CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fava M, Kendler KS (2000) Major depressive disorder. Neuron 28(2):335–341CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fava M, Rush AJ, Alpert JE, Balasubramani GK, Wisniewski SR, Carmin CN, Biggs MM, Zisook S, Leuchter A, Howland R, Warden D, Trivedi MH (2008) Difference in treatment outcome in outpatients with anxious versus nonanxious depression: a STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry 165(3):342–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gotlib IH, Joormann J (2010) Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 6:285–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Guy W (1976) Clinical global impressions. In: Guy W (ed) ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology (Revised). National Institut of Mental Health, Rockville, pp 217–221Google Scholar
  16. Hallion LS, Ruscio AM (2011) A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychol Bull 137(6):940–958CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hamilton M (1960) A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 23:56–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hautzinger M, Bailer M, Worall H, Keller F (1994) Beck-Depressions-Inventar (BDI). Testhandbuch. 1 edn. Hans Huber, BernGoogle Scholar
  19. Johnson SL, Joormann J, Gotlib IH (2007) Does processing of emotional stimuli predict symptomatic improvement and diagnostic recovery from major depression? Emotion (Washington, DC) 7(1):201–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kemp AH, Gordon E, Rush AJ, Williams LM (2008) Improving the prediction of treatment response in depression: integration of clinical, cognitive, psychophysiological, neuroimaging, and genetic measures. CNS Spectr 13(12):1066–1086 (quiz 1087–1068)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (2005) International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. University of Florida, GainesvilleGoogle Scholar
  22. Libkuman TM, Otani H, Kern R, Viger SG, Novak N (2007) Multidimensional normative ratings for the International Affective Picture System. Behav Res Methods 39(2):326–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Mathews A, MacLeod C (2005) Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 1:167–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Mayer B, Muris P, Wilschut M (2011) Fear- and disgust-related covariation bias and eating disorders symptoms in healthy young women. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 42(1):19–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mayer B, Muris P, Kramer Freher N, Stout J, Polak M (2012) Covariation bias for food-related control is associated with eating disorders symptoms in normal adolescents. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 43(4):1008–1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Miskowiak KW, Larsen JE, Harmer CJ, Siebner HR, Kessing LV, Macoveanu J, Vinberg M (2018) Is negative self-referent bias an endophenotype for depression? An fMRI study of emotional self-referent words in twins at high vs. low risk of depression. J Affect Disord 226:267–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Papakostas GI (2012) Surrogate markers of treatment outcome in major depressive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 15(6):841–854CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pauli P, Montoya P, Martz G-E (1996) Covariation bias in panic-prone individuals. J Abnorm Psychol 105(4):658–662CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Roiser JP, Elliott R, Sahakian BJ (2012) Cognitive mechanisms of treatment in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 37(1):117–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Saß H, Wittchen HU, Zaudig M (2003) Diagnostisches und statistisches Manual psychischer Störungen DSM-IV-TR. Hogrefe, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  31. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 59(Suppl 20):22–33 (quiz 34–57)PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE (1970) Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo AltoGoogle Scholar
  33. Stratta P, Tempesta D, Bonanni RL, de Cataldo S, Rossi A (2014) Emotional reactivity in bipolar depressed patients. J Clin Psychol 70(9):860–865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Strauss GP, Allen DN, Jorgensen ML, Cramer SL (2005) Test–retest reliability of standard and emotional stroop tasks: an investigation of color-word and picture-word versions. Assessment 12(3):330–337CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tomarken AJ, Mineka S, Cook M (1989) Fear-relevant selective associations and covariation bias. J Abnorm Psychol 98(4):381–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Wiemer J, Pauli P (2016a) Fear-relevant illusory correlations in different fears and anxiety disorders: a review of the literature. J Anxiety Disord 42:113–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Wiemer J, Pauli P (2016b) How fear-relevant illusory correlations might develop and persist in anxiety disorders: a model of contributing factors. J Anxiety Disord 44:55–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wiemer J, Mühlberger A, Pauli P (2014a) Illusory correlations between neutral and aversive stimuli can be induced by outcome aversiveness. Cogn Emot 28(2):193–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Wiemer J, Schulz S, Reicherts P, Glotzbach-Schoon E, Andreatta M, Pauli P (2014b) Brain activity associated with illusory correlations in animal phobia. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 10(7):969–977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wittchen HU, Zaudig M, Fydrich T (1997) SKID-I: Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV, Achse 1: psychische Störungen. Hogrefe, GöttingenGoogle Scholar
  41. Wittchen HU, Jacobi F, Rehm J, Gustavsson A, Svensson M, Jönsson B, Olesen J, Allgulander C, Alonso J, Faravelli C, Fratiglioni L, Jennum P, Lieb R, Maercker A, van Os J, Preisig M, Salvador-Carulla L, Simon R, Steinhausen HC (2011) The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 21(9):655–679CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Zakzanis KK, Leach L, Kaplan E (1998) On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 11(3):111–119PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Austria, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, Center of Mental HealthUniversity Hospital of WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  2. 2.Department of Biological Psychology, Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Center of Mental HealthUniversity of WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  3. 3.Interdisciplinary Center for Clinical ResearchUniversity Hospital of WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  4. 4.Comprehensive Hearth Failure Center (CHFC)University Hospital of WürzburgWürzburgGermany
  5. 5.Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Medical CenterUniversity of Freiburg, Faculty of MedicineFreiburgGermany

Personalised recommendations