Oecologia

, 157:177

Ecological consequences of the bold–shy continuum: the effect of predator boldness on prey risk

Behavioral Ecology - Original Paper

Abstract

Although the existence of different personality traits within and between animal populations has been relatively well established, the ecological implications of this variation remain neglected. In this study we tested whether differences in the boldness of pairs of three-spined sticklebacks led to differential predation risk in their prey, Chironomidae larvae. Bolder pairs, those that left a refuge and crossed the tank mid-line sooner, ate a greater proportion of prey in 10 min than less bold fish (therefore prey were at a greater per capita risk). Fish crossed the mid-line more rapidly when a larger number of prey were presented, suggesting they accepted greater risk in return for a larger foraging reward. Perception of predation risk also affected the differences between fish in boldness, as larger fish crossed the mid-line sooner after leaving the refuge (larger fish are less at risk from predation). Hence, an interesting trophic interaction occurs, where the risk experienced by the chironomid larvae is determined by the risk perceived by their predators. Through the variation generated by boldness, a form of behaviourally mediated trophic cascade can occur within (as well as between) communities.

Keywords

Personality Predation risk Three-spined stickleback Trophic cascade 

References

  1. Beckerman AP, Uriarte M, Schmitz OJ (1997) Experimental evidence for a behavior-mediated trophic cascade in a terrestrial food chain. PNAS 94(20):10735–10738CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Both C, Dingemanse NJ, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM (2005) Pairs of extreme personalities have highest reproductive success. J Anim Ecol 74:667–674CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Botham MS, Kerfoot CJ, Louca V, Krause J (2006) The effects of different predator species on antipredator behavior in the Trinidadian guppy, Poecilia reticulata. Naturwissenschaften 93:431–439CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown C, Jones F, Braithwaite V (2005) In situ examination of boldness–shyness traits in the tropical poeciliid, Brachyraphis episcopi. Anim Behav 70:1003–1009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Coleman K, Wilson DS (1998) Shyness and boldness in pumpkinseed sunfish: individual differences are context-specific. Anim Behav 56:927–936CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Cresswell W (1994) Flocking is an effective anti-predation strategy in redshanks, Tringa totanus. Anim Behav 47:433–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, van Noordwijk AJ, Rutten AL, Drent PJ (2003) Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits (Parus major). Proc R Soc B 270:741–747CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  8. Dingemanse NJ, Both C, Drent PJ, Tinbergen JM (2004) Fitness consequences of avian personalities in a fluctuating environment. Proc R Soc B 271:847–852CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  9. FitzGibbon CD (1990) Mixed species grouping in Thomson and Grant gazelles—the anti-predatory benefits. Anim Behav 39:1116–1126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fraser DF, Gilliam JF, Daley MJ, Le AN, Skalski GT (2001) Explaining leptokurtic movement distributions: intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration. Am Nat 158:124–135CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Genkai-Kato M (2007) Macrophyte refuges, prey behaviour and trophic interactions: consequences for lake water clarity. Ecol Lett 10(2):105–114CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Godin J-GJ, Smith SA (1988) A fitness cost of foraging in the guppy. Nature 333:69–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Hölker F, Steif P (2005) Adaptive behaviour of chironomid larvae (Chironomus riparius) in response to chemical stimuli from predators and resource density. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 58:256–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Huang C, Sih A (1991) Experimental studies on direct and indirect interactions in a three trophic-level stream system. Oecologia 85:530–536CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Hugie DM, Dill LM (1994) Fish and game—a game-theoretic approach to habitat selection by predators and prey. J Fish Biol 45:151–169Google Scholar
  16. Ioannou CC, Krause J (2008) Searching for prey—the effect of group size and number. Anim Behav 75(4):1383–1388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Krause J, Ruxton GD (2002) Living in groups. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  18. Krause J, Loader SP, McDermott J, Ruxton GD (1998) Refuge use by fish as a function of body length-related metabolic expenditure and predation risks. Proc R Soc B 265(1413):2373–2379CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Lima SL (1998) Stress and decision making under the risk of predation: recent developments from behavioral, reproductive, and ecological perspectives. Adv Stud Behav 27:215–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lima SL (2002) Putting predators back into behavioural predator–prey interactions. Trends Ecol Evol 17:70–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McGill BJ, Mittelbach GG (2006) An allometric vision and motion model to predict prey encounter rates. Evol Ecol Res 8:691–701Google Scholar
  22. Réale D, Festa-Bianchet M (2003) Predator-induced selection on temperament in bighorn ewes. Anim Behav 65:463–470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Réale D, Gallant BY, LeBlanc M, Festa-Bianchet M (2000) Consistency of temperament in bighorn ewes and correlates with behaviour and life history. Anim Behav 60:589–597CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT, Dingemanse NJ (2007) Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution. Biol Rev 82(2):291–318CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Sih A (1998) Game theory and predator–prey response races. In: Dugatkin LA, Reeve HK (eds) Advances in game theory and the study of animal behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 221–238Google Scholar
  26. Sih A, Bell AM, Johnson JC (2004) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends Ecol Evol 19:372–378CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Sogard SM (1997) Size-selective mortality in the juvenile stage of teleost fishes: a review. Bull Mar Sci 60:1129–1157Google Scholar
  28. Turner GF, Pitcher J (1986) Attack abatement: a model for group protection by combined avoidance and dilution. Am Nat 128:228–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ward AJW, Thomas P, Hart PJB, Krause J (2004) Correlates of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55:561–568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Wilson DS, Clark AB, Coleman K, Dearstyne T (1994) Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Trends Ecol Evol 9:442–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institute of Integrative and Comparative BiologyUniversity of LeedsLeedsUK

Personalised recommendations