Oecologia

, Volume 145, Issue 1, pp 41–52 | Cite as

Gregariousness increases brain size in ungulates

Population Ecology

Abstract

The brain’s main function is to organise the physiological and behavioural responses to environmental and social challenges in order to keep the organism alive. Here, we studied the effects that gregariousness (as a measurement of sociality), dietary habits, gestation length and sex have on brain size of extant ungulates. The analysis controlled for the effects of phylogeny and for random variability implicit in the data set. We tested the following groups of hypotheses: (1) Social brain hypothesis—gregarious species are more likely to have larger brains than non-gregarious species because the former are subjected to demanding and complex social interactions; (2) Ecological hypothesis—dietary habits impose challenging cognitive tasks associated with finding and manipulating food (foraging strategy); (3) Developmental hypotheses (a) energy strategy: selection for larger brains operates, primarily, on maternal metabolic turnover (i.e. gestation length) in relation to food quality because the majority of the brain’s growth takes place in utero, and finally (b) sex hypothesis: females are expected to have larger brains than males, relative to body size, because of the differential growth rates of the soma and brain between the sexes. We found that, after adjusting for body mass, gregariousness and gestation length explained most of the variation in brain mass across the ungulate species studied. Larger species had larger brains; gregarious species and those with longer gestation lengths, relative to body mass, had larger brains than non-gregarious species and those with shorter gestation lengths. The effect of diet was negligible and subrogated by gestation length, and sex had no significant effect on brain size. The ultimate cause that could have triggered the co-evolution between gestation length and brain size remains unclear.

Keywords

Evolution Gestation Browser Grazer Sex 

References

  1. Alados CL, Escos J (1987) Relationships between movement rate, agonistic displacements and forage availability in Spanish ibexes (Capra pyrenaica). Biol Behav 12:245–255Google Scholar
  2. Altmann PL, Dittmer DS (1972) Biology Data Book. Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology, Bethesda, MDGoogle Scholar
  3. Anderson AE, Medin DE, Bowden DC (1974) Growth & morphometry of the carcass, selected bones, organs and glands of mule deer. Wildl Monogr 39:1–122Google Scholar
  4. Barton RA, Harvey PH (2000) Mosaic evolution of brain structure in mammals. Nature 405:1055–1058PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bauchot R (1985) L’encéphalisation chez les carnivores et les Artiodactyles. Mammalia 49:559–572CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bauchot R, Stephan H (1964) Le poids encéphalique chez les insectivores malagaches. Acta zool Stockh 45:63–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bauchot R, Stephan H (1966) Données nouvelles sur l’encephalisation des insectivores et des prosimièns. Mammalia 33:225–275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beauchamp G (2001) Should vigilance always decrease with group size? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:47–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Beauchamp G, Ruxton GD (2003) Changes in vigilance with group size under scramble competition. Am Nat 161:672–675PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Blomberg SP, Garland T, Ives AR (2003) Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution 57:717–745PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Brashares JS, Garland T, Arcese P (2000) Phylogenetic analysis of coadaptation in behaviour, diet, and body size in the African antelope. Behav Ecol 11:452–463CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Budeau DA, Verts BJ (1986) Relative brain size and structural complexity of habitats of Chipmunks. J Mammal 67:579–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bunnell FL, Olsen NA (1976) Weights and growth of Dall sheep in Kluane Park Reserve, Yukon Territory. Can Field Nat 90:157–162Google Scholar
  14. Caraco T (1981) Risk-sensitivity and foraging groups. Ecology 62:527–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Clutton-Brock TH (1991) The evolution of parental care. Princeton University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  16. Clutton-Brock TH, Harvey PH (1980) Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool 190:309–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Clutton-Brock TH, Guinness FE, Albon SD (1982) Red deer: behaviour and ecology of two sexes. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  18. Cowlishaw G, Lawes MJ, Lightbody M, Martin A, Pettifor R, Rowcliffe JM (2004) A simple rule for the costs of vigilance: empirical evidence from a social forager. In: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. Biol Sci 271:27–33Google Scholar
  19. Corbet GB, Hill JE (1986) A world list of mammalian species. British Museum (Natural History), LondonGoogle Scholar
  20. Crile G, Quiring DP (1940) A record of the body weight and certain organ and gland weights of 3690 animals. Ohio J Sci 40:219–259Google Scholar
  21. Deacon TW (1990) Problems of ontogeny and phylogeny in brain-size evolution. Int J Primatol 11:237–282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Deacon TW (1998) The symbolic species: the co-evolution of language and the brain. WW Norton & Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  23. Demment MW, van Soest PJ (1985) A nutritional explanation for body-size patterns of ruminant and nonruminant herbivores. Am Nat 125:641–672CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dunbar RIM (1992) Neocortex size as a constraint on group-size in Primates. J Hum Evol 22:469–493CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Dunbar RIM (1995) Neocortex size and group-size in Primates—a test of the hypothesis. J Hum Evol 28:287–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Dunbar RIM (1998) The social brain hypothesis. Evol Anthropol 6:178–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Dunbar RIM, Bever J (1998) Neocortex size predicts group size in carnivores and some insectivores. Ethology 104:695–708CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Dutoit JT, Provenza FD, Nastis A (1991) Conditioned taste-aversions—how sick must a ruminant get before it learns about toxicity in foods. Appl Anim Behav Sci 30:35–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ebinger P (1974) A cytoarchitectonic volumetric comparison of brains in wild and domestic sheep. Zeitschrift fuer Anatomie und Entwicklungsgeschichte 144:267–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Eisenberg JF (1981) The mammalian radiations. University of Chicago Press, ChicagoGoogle Scholar
  31. Eisenberg JF, Wilson DE (1978) Relative brain size and feeding strategies in the Chiroptera. Evolution 32:740–751CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Farnsworth KD, Focardi S, Beecham JA (2002) Grassland-herbivore interactions: how do grazers coexist? Am Nat 159:24–39PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Fortin D (2003) Searching behavior and use of sampling information by free- ranging bison (Bos bison). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 54:194–203Google Scholar
  34. Fritz H, deGarineWichatitsky M (1996) Foraging in a social antelope: effects of group size on foraging choices and resource perception in impala. J Anim Ecol 65:736–742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Gagnon M, Chew AE (2000) Dietary preferences in extant African bovidae. J Mammal 81:490–511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Garland T, Janis CM (1993) Does metatarsal femur ratio predict maximal running speed in cursorial mammals. J Zool 229:133–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Garland T, Harvey PH, Ives AR (1992) Procedures for the analysis of comparative data using phylogenetically independent contrasts. Syst Biol 41:18–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gatesy J, Amato G, Vrba E, Schaller G, DeSalle R (1997) A cladistic analysis of mitochondrial ribosomal dna from the bovidae. Mol Phylogenet Evol 7:303–319PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. GenStat 6.1. (2002) GenStat 6.1. reference manual. VSN International Ltd, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  40. Gentry AW (1992) The subfamilies and tribes of the family bovidae. Mamm Rev 22:1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Gentry AW, Hooker JJ (1988) The phylogeny of the Artiodactyla. In: Benton MJ (ed) The phylogeny and classification of the Tetrapods, Volume 2: Mammals. Systematics Association Special Volume 35B Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 235–272Google Scholar
  42. Georgiadis N (1985) Growth patterns, sexual dimorphism and reproduction in african ruminants. Afr J Ecol 23:75–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gibbons A (1998) Solving the brain’s energy crisis. Science 280:1345–1347PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Gittleman JL (1986) Carnivore brain size, behavioral ecology, and phylogeny. J Mammal 67:23–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Goldspink CR, Holland RK, Sweet G, Stewart L (2002) A note on group sizes of oribi (Ourebia ourebi, Zimmermann, 1783) from two contrasting sites in Zambia, with and without predation. Afr J Ecol 40:372–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Gordon IJ, Illius AW (1994) The functional-significance of the browser-grazer dichotomy in african ruminants. Oecologia 98:167–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Gordon IJ, Illius AW (1996) The nutritional ecology of african ruminants—a reinterpretation. J Animal Ecol 65:18–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Grafen A (1989) The phylogenetic regression. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London Series. B Biol Sci 326:119–157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Harvey PH, Pagel MD (1991) The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  50. Hemmer H (1979) Socialization by intelligence: social behavior in carnivores as a function of relative brain size and environment. Carnivore 2:102–105Google Scholar
  51. Herre W, Thiede U (1965) Studien an Gehirnen südamerikanischer Tylopoden. Zoologischer Anzeiger 81:155–176Google Scholar
  52. Hofman MA (1983) Evolution of the brain in neonatal and adult placental mammals: a theoretical approach. J Theoretic Biol 105:317–322CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Hofmann RR (1989) Evolutionary steps of ecophysiological adaptation and diversification of ruminants—a comparative view of their digestive-system. Oecologia 78:443–457CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Honda K, Tatsukawa R, Miura S (1987) Body and Organ Weights of Free-Ranging Japanese Serow. J Wildl Manag 51:678–680CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Hrdlicka A (1905) Brain weight in vertebrates. Smithsonian miscellaneous collections 48:89–112Google Scholar
  56. Iwaniuk AN, Arnold KE (2004) Is cooperative breeding associated with bigger brains? A comparative test in the Corvida (Passeriformes). Ethology 110:203–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Janis CM, Scott KM (1988) The phylogeny of the Ruminantia (Artiodactyla, Mammalia). In: Benton MJ (ed) The phylogeny and classification of the tetrapods, Volume 2: Mammals. Systematics Association Special Volume 35B Clarendon Press, Oxford, pp 273–282Google Scholar
  58. Janis CM, Damuth J, Theodor JM (2000) Miocene ungulates and terrestrial primary productivity: where have all the browsers gone? In: Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97:7899–7904Google Scholar
  59. Kingdon J (1982) East African mammals. An atlas of evolution in Africa. Bovids. Academic Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  60. Klemmt L (1960) Quantitative Untersuchungen an Apodemus sylvaticus L. 1758. Zoologischer Anzeiger 165:275Google Scholar
  61. Krasinska M, Krasinski ZA (2002) Body mass and measurements of the European bison during postnatal development. Acta Theriologica 47:85–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Kretschmann HJ (1966) Über die Cerebralisation eines Nestflüchters (Acomys cahirinus dimidiatus Cretzschmar 1826) im Vergleich mit Nesthockern (Albiomanus, Apodemus sylvaticus, Linnaeus 1758, und Albinoratte). Morphologisches Jahrbuch 109:376–410Google Scholar
  63. Kruska D (1973) Cerebralisation, Hirnevolution und domestikationsbedingte Hirngrößenänderungen innerhalb der Ordnung Perissodactyla Owen, 1848 und ein Vergleich mit der Ordnung Artiodactyla Owen, 1848. Zeitschrift fuer Zoologische Systematik und Evolutionsforschung 11:81–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Kruska D (1987) How fast can total brain size change in mammals? J fuer Hirnforschung 28:59–70Google Scholar
  65. Mace GM (1979) The evolutionary ecology of small mammals. PhD. Thesis, University of Sussex.Google Scholar
  66. Mace GM, Harvey PH, Clutton-Brock TH (1980) Is brain size an ecological variable? Trends NeuroSci 1980:193–196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Mace GM, Harvey PH, Clutton-Brock TH (1981) Brain size and ecology in small mammals. J Zool 193:333–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Martin RD. Human brain evolution in an ecological context, New York, 1983. Fifty-second James Arthur lecture on the evolution of the human brain, 1–58. 1983. New York, American Museum of Natural History.Google Scholar
  69. Martin RD (1981) Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature 293:57–60PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Martin RD (1984) Body size, brain size and feeding strategies in primates. In: Chivers DJ, Wood B, Bilsborough A (eds) Food acquisition and processing in Primates. Plenum Press, London, pp 73–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Martin RD, Harvey PH (1985) Brain size allometry: ontogeny and phylogeny. In: Jungers WL (ed) Size and scaling in primate biology. Plenum Press, New York, pp 147–173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Mirza SN, Provenza FD (1992) Effects of age and conditions of exposure on maternally mediated food selection by lambs. Appl Anim Behav Sci 33:35–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Molvar EM, Bowyer RT (1994) Costs and benefits of group living in a recently social ungulate—the alaskan moose. J Mammal 75:621–630CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Mysterud A (1998) The relative roles of body size and feeding type on activity time of temperate ruminants. Oecologia 113:442–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Nowak RM (1999) Walker’s mammals of the world. The Johns Hopkins University Press, BaltimoreGoogle Scholar
  76. Oboussier H (1974a) Beiträge zur Kenntnis afrikanischer Gezellen unter besonderer Berücksichtigung des Körperbaus, der Hypophyse und der Hirngröße. Mitteilungen aus dem Hamburgischen Zoologischen Museum und Institut 71:235–257Google Scholar
  77. Oboussier H (1974b) Zur Kenntnis der Hippotraginae (Bovidae, Mammalia) unter besonderer Berücksichtigung von Körperbau, Hypophyse und Hirn. Mitteilungen aus dem Hamburgischen Zoologischen Museum und Institut 71:203–233Google Scholar
  78. Ovadia O, Dohna HZ (2003) The effect of intra- and interspecific aggression on patch residence time in Negev Desert gerbils: a competing risk analysis. Behav Ecol 14:583–591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Owen-Smith N (1991) Grazers and browsers: ecological and social contrasts among African ruminants. In: Ongules/ungulates 91 pp 175–181Google Scholar
  80. Owen-Smith N (1997) Distinctive features of the nutritional ecology of browsing versus grazing ruminants. Zeitschrift Fur Saugetierkunde-International Journal of Mammalian Biology 62:176–191Google Scholar
  81. Pagel MD (1992) A method for the analysis of comparative data. J Theoretic Biol 156:431–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Pagel MD, Harvey PH (1988) How mammals produce larger-brained offspring. Evolution 42:948–957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Peltier TC, Barboza PS (2003) Growth in an Arctic grazer: effects of sex and dietary nitrogen on yearling muskoxen. J Mammal 84:915–925CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ (1998) The influence of sexual dimorphism in body size and mouth morphology on diet selection and sexual segregation in cervids. Acta Veterinaria Hungarica 46:357–367PubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ (1999) The relative roles of phylogeny, body size and feeding style on the activity time of temperate ruminants: a reanalysis. Oecologia 120:193–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ (2000) Differences in body mass and oral morphology between the sexes in the Artiodactyla: evolutionary relationships with sexual segregation. Evol Ecol Res 2:667–684Google Scholar
  87. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ (2001) Relationships between oral morphology and feeding style in the Ungulata: a phylogenetically controlled evaluation. In: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. Biol Sci 268:1023–1032Google Scholar
  88. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ, Illius AW (2001a) Phylogenetic analysis of stomach adaptation in digestive strategies in African ruminants. Oecologia 129:498–508CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ, Nores C (2001b) Evolutionary transitions among feeding styles and habitats in ungulates. Evol Ecol Res 3:221–230Google Scholar
  90. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Gordon IJ, Pagel M (2002) The origins of sexual dimorphism in body size in ungulates. Evolution 56:1276–1285PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Pérez-Barbería FJ, Elston DA, Gordon IJ, Illius AW (2004) The evolution of phylogenetic differences in the efficiency of digestion in ruminants. In: Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B. 271:1081–1090Google Scholar
  92. Pullian HR, Caraco T (1984) Living in groups: is there optimal group size? In: Krebs JR, Davies Balckwell NB (eds) Behavioural Ecology. An Evolutionary Approach Scientific Publications, pp 122–147Google Scholar
  93. Quiring DP (1938) A comparison of certain gland, organ and body weights in some African ungulates and the African elephant. Growth 2:335–346Google Scholar
  94. Randi E, Mucci N, Claro-Hergueta F, Bonnet A, Douzery EJP (2001) A mitochondrial DNA control region phylogeny of the Cervinae: speciation in Cervus and implications for conservation. Anim Conserv 4:1–11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Ringberg TM, White RG, Holleman DF, Luick JR (1981) Body Growth and Carcass Composition of Lean Reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus L) from Birth to Sexual Maturity. Can J Zool Rev Can Zool 59:1040–1044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Röhrs M, Ebinger P (1978) Die Beurteilung von Hirngrößenunterschieden zwischen Wild- und Haustieren. Zeitschrift fuer Zoologische Systematik und Evolutionsforschung 16:1–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Ronnefeld U (1970) Morphologische und quantitative Neocortexuntersuchungen bei Boviden, ein Beitrag zur Phylogenie dieser Familie. I. Formen mittlerer Körpergrösse (25 kg bis 75 kg). Gegenbaurs morphologisches Jahrbuch 115:161–230Google Scholar
  98. Rushton JP, Ankney CD (1996) Brain size and cognitive ability: correlations with age, sex, social class, and race. Psychonomic Bull Rev 3:21–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Sacher GA, Staffeldt EF (1974) Relation of gestion time to brain weight for placental mammals: implications for the theory of vertebrate growth. Am Nat 108:593–615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Sigmund L (1981) Morphometrische untersuchungen an Gehirnen der Wiederkäuer (Ruminantia, Artiodactyla, Mammalia). Die Hirn-Körpergewichtsbeziehung der Hirschferkel (Tragulidae). Acta Universitatis Carolinae - Biologica 1979:447–463Google Scholar
  101. Spector WS (1956) Handbook of biological data. W.B. Saunders Co., Philadelphia and LondonGoogle Scholar
  102. Stamps JA (1993) Sexual size dimorphism in species with asymptotic growth after maturity. Biol J Linn Soc 50:123–145CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (1989) Using multivariate statistics. Harper Collins Publishers Inc, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  104. Underwood R (1982) Vigilance behaviour in grazing African antelopes. Behaviour 79:81–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Van Wieren SE (1996) Digestive strategies in ruminants and nonruminants. University of Wageningen, WageningenGoogle Scholar
  106. van Soest PJ (1996) Allometry and ecology of feeding-behavior and digestive capacity in herbivores—a review. Zoo Biol 15:455–479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. von Bonin G (1937) Brain weight and body weight of mammals. J Gen Physiol 16:379–389Google Scholar
  108. von Tyska H (1966) Das Großhirnfurchenbild als Merkmal der Evolution Untersuchungen an Boviden I. Mitteilungen aus dem Hamburgischen Zoologischen Museum und Institut 63:121–158Google Scholar
  109. Wemmer C, Wilson DE (1987) Cervid brain size and natural history. In: Wemmer CM (ed) Biology and management of the Cervidae. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington DC and London, pp 189–199Google Scholar
  110. White RG, Trudell J (1980) Patterns of herbivory and nutrient intake of reindeer grazing tundra vegetation. In: Reimers E, Gaare A, Skjenneberg S (eds) Proceedings of the 2nd International Reindeer/Caribou symposium. Trondheim, Direktoratet for vilt og ferskvannsfisk pp 180–195Google Scholar
  111. Willner LA, Martin RD (1985) Some basic principles of mammalian sexual dimophism. In: Ghesquiere J, Martin RD, Newcombe F (eds) Human Sexual Dimorphism Taylor & Francis, London, Philadelphia, pp 1–42Google Scholar
  112. de Winter W, Oxnard CE (2001) Evolutionary radiations and convergences in the structural organization of mammalian brains. Nature 409:710–714PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The Macaulay Institute, CraigiebucklerAberdeenUK
  2. 2.Sustainable Rangelands SystemsCSIRO – Davies Laboratory Australia

Personalised recommendations