Brain Structure and Function

, Volume 219, Issue 3, pp 1129–1138 | Cite as

Differences between endogenous and exogenous emotion inhibition in the human brain

Original Article

Abstract

The regulation of emotions is an integral part of our mental health. It has only recently been investigated using brain imaging techniques. In most studies, participants are instructed by a cue to inhibit a specific emotional reaction. The aim of the present study was to investigate the alternative situation where a person decides to inhibit an emotion as an act of endogenous self-control. Healthy participants viewed highly arousing pictures with negative valence. In the endogenous condition, participants could freely choose on each trial to inhibit or feel the emotions elicited by the picture. In an exogenous condition, a visual cue instructed them to either feel or inhibit the emotion elicited by the picture. Participants’ subjective ratings of intensity of experienced emotion showed an interaction effect between source of control (endogenous/exogenous) and feel/inhibit based on a stronger modulation between feel and inhibition for the endogenous compared to the exogenous condition. Endogenous inhibition of emotions was associated with dorso-medial prefrontal cortex activation, whereas exogenous inhibition was found associated with lateral prefrontal cortex activation. Thus, the brain regions for both endogenous and exogenous inhibition of emotion are highly similar to those for inhibition of motor actions in Brass and Haggard (J Neurosci 27:9141–9145, 2007), Kühn et al. (Hum Brain Mapp 30:2834–2843, 2009). Functional connectivity analyses showed that dorsofrontomedial cortex exerts greater control onto pre-supplementary motor area during endogenous inhibition compared to endogenous feel. This functional dissociation between an endogenous, fronto-medial and an exogenous, fronto-lateral inhibition centre has important implications for our understanding of emotion regulation in health and psychopathology.

Keywords

Inhibition Volition Emotion regulation fMRI Dorsofrontomedial cortex 

References

  1. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S (2010) Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 30:217–237PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aron AR, Poldrack RA (2005) The cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 57:1285–1292PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Aron AR, Poldrack RA (2006) Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci 26:2424–2433PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA (2004) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 8:170–177PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beauregard M, Levesque J, Bourgouin P (2001) Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. J Neurosci 21:RC165PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Brass M, Haggard P (2007) To do or not to do: the neural signature of self-control. J Neurosci 27:9141–9145PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brett M, Anton J-C, Valabregue R, Poline JB (2002) Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox (abstract) Presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional mapping of the human brain, June 2–6Google Scholar
  8. Carver CS, Scheier ME (1981) Attention and self-control: a control theory approach to human behavior. Springer, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Davidson RJ (2002) Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Biol Psychiatry 51:68–80PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Descartes R (1649) Les Passions de l’âme. (The Passions of the Soul). In: Cottingham J, Stoothoff R, Murdoch D, Kenny A (eds) The philosophical writings of descartes, vol I. Cambridge University, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  11. Dimberg U (1982) Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology 19:643–647PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dimberg U, Thunberg M, Grunedal S (2002) Facial reactions to emotional stimuli: automatically controlled emotional responses. Cogn Emot 16:449–471CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Drabant EM, McRae K, Manuck SB, Hariri AR, Gross JJ (2009) Individual differences in typical reappraisal use predict amygdala and prefrontal responses. Biol Psychiatry 65:367–373PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Eifert GH, Heffner M (2003) The effects of acceptance versus control contexts on avoidance of panic-related symptoms. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 34:293–312PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Eippert F, Veit R, Weiskopf N, Erb M, Birbaumer N, Anders S (2007) Regulation of emotional responses elicited by threat-related stimuli. Hum Brain Mapp 28:409–423PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ (1997) Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging. NeuroImage 6:218–229PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Goldberg G (1985) Supplementary motor area structure and function: review and hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci 8:567–616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Goldin PR, McRea K, Ramel W, Gross JJ (2008) The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biol Psychiatry 63:577–586PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gross JJ (1998) The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev General Psychol 2:271–299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hagger MS, Wood C, Stiff C, Chatzisarantis NL (2010) Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 136:495–525PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. James W (1884) What is an emotion? Mind 9:188–205CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kalisch R, Wiech K, Herrmann K, Dolan RJ (2006) Neural correlates of self-distraction from anxiety and a process model of cognitive emotion regulation. J Cogn Neurosci 18:1266–1276PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kim SH, Hamann S (2007) Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. J Cogn Neurosci 19:776–798PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kühn S, Haggard P, Brass M (2009) Intentional inhibition: how the “veto-area” exerts control. Hum Brain Mapp 30:2834–2843PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Lane RD, Nadel L (2000) Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford UP, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  26. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (2005) International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report A-6. University of Florida, Gainesville, FLGoogle Scholar
  27. Lange C (1887) Ueber Gemüthsbewegungen. Thomas, LeipzigGoogle Scholar
  28. Lecrubier Y, Sheehan D, Weiller E, Amorim P, Bonora I, Sheehan K et al (1997) The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview. (MINI), a short diagnostic interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur Psychiatry 12:224–231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Levesque J, Eugene F, Joanette Y, Paquette V, Mensour B, Beaudoin G, Leroux JM, Bourgouin P, Beauregard M (2003) Neural circuitry underlying voluntary suppression of sadness. Biol Psychiatry 53:502–510PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Li CR, Huang C, Constable RT, Sinha R (2006) Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J Neurosci 26:186–192PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Libet B, Wright EW, Gleason C A (1983) Preparation- or intention-to-act, in relation to pre-event potentials recorded at the vertex. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 56:367–372PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Burdette JB, Kraft RA (2003) An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage 19:1233–1239PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Burdette JH (2004) Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the talairach atlas. NeuroImage 21:450–455PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Menon V, Adleman NE, White CD, Glover GH, Reiss AL (2001) Error-related brain activation during a Go/NoGo response inhibition task. Hum Brain Mapp 12:131–143PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Morrison I, Poliakoff E, Gordon L, Downing P (2007) Response-specific effects of pain observation on motor behaviour. Cognition 104:407–416PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ochsner KN, Gross JJ (2005) The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 9:242–249PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, Robertson ER, Chopra S, Gabrieli JDE, Gross JJ (2004) For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. NeuroImage 23:483–499PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Oldfield RC (1971) The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Oliveri M, Babiloni C, Filippi MM, Caltagirone C et al (2003) Influence of the supplementary motor area on primary motor cortex excitability during movements triggered by neutral or emotionally unpleasant visual cues. Exp Brain Res 149:214–221PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Passingham RE, Bengtsson SL, Lau HC (2010) Medial frontal cortex: from self- generated action to reflection on one’s own performance. Trends Cogn Sci 14:16–21PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Phan KL, Fitzgerald DA, Nathan PJ, Moore GJ, Uhde TW, Tancer ME (2005) Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 57:210–219PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sato W, Yoshikawa S (2007) Spontaneous facial mimicry in response to dynamic facial expressions. Cognition 104:1–18PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Simmonds DJ, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH (2008) Meta-analysis of Go/NoGo tasks demonstrating that fMRI activation associated with response inhibition is task dependent. Neuropsychologia 46:224–232PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Spengler S, von Cramon DY, Brass M (2009) Was it me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage 46:290–298PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Staudinger MR, Erk S, Abler B, Walter H (2009) Cognitive reappraisal modulates expected value and prediction error encoding in the ventral striatum. NeuroImage 47:713–721PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. van Reekum CM, Johnstone T, Urry HL, Thurow ME, Schaefer HS, Alexander AL, Davidson RJ (2007) Gaze fixations predict brain activation during the voluntary regulation of picture-induced negative affect. NeuroImage 36:1041–1055PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wild B, Erb M, Bartels M (2001) Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Res 102:109–124PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Experimental Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational SciencesGhent Institute for Functional and Metabolic Imaging, Ghent UniversityGhentBelgium
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyInstitute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College LondonLondonUK

Personalised recommendations