Advertisement

Yellow is for safety: perceptual and affective perspectives

  • Kesong HuEmail author
  • Eve De Rosa
  • Adam K. AndersonEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

Is there a link between color and safety? Yellow is often used in safety contexts. Using the Singapore accident record datasets, Ho et al. provided evidence that yellow taxis have fewer accidents than blue taxis (Ho et al. in Proc Natl Acad Sci 114(12):3074–3078, 2017). Does yellow differentially influence attention and action and if so is this related to purely visual or affective factors? Here, we examined the visual priority of yellow relative to luminance matched colors at opposing ends of the wavelength spectrum (i.e., red and blue), using a temporal order judgment task, between color pairs. Despite being matched in arousal, when yellow and blue were pitted against each other, yellow was consistently seen as occurring first, even when objectively appearing second at short stimulus onset asynchronies. Despite being matched in valence, yellow again showed a larger temporal priority when it was pitted against red. Yellow temporal priority bias was modulated by individual differences in regulatory focus, highlighting a potential affective-motivational origin. These results support that yellow is a safety color, having a temporal advantage, and further evidence that colors have special influences on cognition, perception and behavior.

Notes

Acknowledgements

We thank Celine Cammarata, and Ross Markello for assistance with subject recruitment and data collection. We also thank Juan Lupiáñez for constructive suggestions on an earlier draft of the paper, and all participants for their contribution.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding authors upon request.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Abu-Akel, A., & Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 49(11), 2971–2984.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker, M. (2015). Over half of psychology studies fail reproducibility test. Nature News.  https://doi.org/10.1038/nature.2015.18248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Braun, C. C., Mine, P. B., & Silver, N. C. (1995). The influence of color on warning label perceptions. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, 15(3), 179–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cai, M., Stetson, C., & Eagleman, D. M. (2012). A neural model for temporal order judgments and their active recalibration: A common mechanism for space and time? Frontiers in psychology, 3, 470.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cajochen, C. (2007). Alerting effects of light. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 11(6), 453–464.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carter, R. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2013). A nexus model of the temporal–parietal junction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 328–336.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., & Shimojo, S. (2006). Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision. Biology Letters, 2(2), 217–221.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clarke, T., & Costall, A. (2008). The emotional connotations of color: a qualitative investigation. Color Research & Application, 33(5), 406–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Davis, B., Christie, J., & Rorden, C. (2009). Temporal order judgments activate temporal parietal junction. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(10), 3182–3188.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Elliot, A. J. (2015). Color and psychological functioning: a review of theoretical and empirical work. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 368.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2012). Color-in-context theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 61–125.Google Scholar
  12. Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2014). Color psychology: effects of perceiving color on psychological functioning in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 95–120.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Goldstein, K. (1942). Some experimental observations concerning the influence of colors on the function of the organism. American Journal of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 21(3), 147–151.Google Scholar
  14. Gorn, G. J., Chattopadhyay, A., Sengupta, J., & Tripathi, S. (2004). Waiting for the web: how screen color affects time perception. Journal of Marketing Research, 41(2), 215–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Guilford, J. P. (1934). The affective value of color as a function of hue, tint, and chroma. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17(3), 342–370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hecht, S. (1924). The visibility of the spectrum. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 9(3), 211–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 1–46). Cambridge, MA: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  19. Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson, L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement orientations from subjective histories of success: promotion pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(1), 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Hill, R. A., & Barton, R. A. (2005). Psychology: red enhances human performance in contests. Nature, 435(7040), 293.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Ho, T.-H., Chong, J. K., & Xia, X. (2017). Yellow taxis have fewer accidents than blue taxis because yellow is more visible than blue. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(12), 3074–3078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hu, K. (2018). Neural activity to threat in ventromedial prefrontal cortex correlates with individual differences in anxiety and reward processing. Neuropsychologia, 117, 566–573.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hu, K. (2019). Reward priming eliminates color-driven affect in perception. Psychological Research, 83, 321–331.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hu, K., De Rosa, E., & Anderson, A. (2018). Differential temporal salience of earning and saving. Nature Communications, 9, 2843.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hu, K., De Rosa, E., & Anderson, A. (2019). The effect of color on the brain. American Psychologist (under review).Google Scholar
  26. Hu, K., Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2013). Interactions between reward and threat during visual processing. Neuropsychologia, 51(9), 1763–1772.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hu, K., Samuel, A. G., & Chan, A. S. (2011). Eliminating inhibition of return by changing salient nonspatial attributes in a complex environment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 35–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Jennings, J. R., & Wood, C. C. (1976). The e-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures analyses of variance. Psychophysiology, 13, 277–278.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lahrmann, H., Madsen, T. K. O., Olesen, A. V., Madsen, J. C. O., & Hels, T. (2018). The effect of a yellow bicycle jacket on cyclist accidents. Safety Science, 108, 209–217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lazareva, O. F., Shimizu, T., & Wasserman, E. A. (Eds.) (2012). How animals see the world: Comparative behavior, biology, and evolution of vision. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Megias, A., Maldonado, A., Candido, A., & Catena, A. (2011a). Emotional modulation of urgent and evaluative behaviors in risky driving scenarios. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43(3), 813–817.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Megias, A., Maldonado, A., Catena, A., Di Stasi, L. L., Serrano, J., & Candido, A. (2011b). Modulation of attention and urgent decisions by affect-laden roadside advertisement in risky driving scenarios. Safety Science, 49(10), 1388–1393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mehta, R., & Zhu, R. J. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323(5918), 1226–1229.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Meier, B. P., D’agostino, P. R., Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., & Wilkowski, B. M. (2012). Color in context: Psychological context moderates the influence of red on approach-and avoidance-motivated behavior. PloS One, 7(7), e40333.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Nakshian, J. S. (1964). The effects of red and green surroundings on behavior. The Journal of General Psychology, 70(1), 143–161.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., Xu, Z., & Prado-León, L. R. (2013). Music–color associations are mediated by emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(22), 8836–8841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Shore, D. I., Spence, C., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry. Psychological Science, 12(3), 205–212.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Theeuwes, J., & der Burg, E. (2013). Priming makes a stimulus more salient. Journal of Vision, 13(3), 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Thompson, E., Palacios, A., & Varela, F. J. (1992). Ways of coloring: comparative color vision as a case study for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(1), 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Titchener, E. B. (1908). Lectures on the elementary psychology of feeling and attention. Macmillan.Google Scholar
  41. Wager, T. D., Keller, M. C., Lacey, S. C., & Jonides, J. (2005). Increased sensitivity in neuroimaging analyses using robust regression. Neuroimage, 26(1), 99–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. West, G. L., Anderson, A. K., Bedwell, J. S., & Pratt, J. (2010). Red diffuse light suppresses the accelerated perception of fear. Psychological Science, 21(7), 992–999.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. West, G. L., Anderson, A. A. K., & Pratt, J. (2009). Motivationally significant stimuli show visual prior entry: evidence for attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1032–1042.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Wilcox, R. R. (2005). Outlier detection. In B. S. Everitt & D. C. Howell (Eds.), Encyclopedia of statistics in behavioral science (Vol. 3, pp. 1497–1498). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  45. Yarrow, K., Jahn, N., Durant, S., & Arnold, D. H. (2011). Shifts of criteria or neural timing? The assumptions underlying timing perception studies. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1518–1531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Human DevelopmentCornell UniversityIthacaUSA
  2. 2.Human Neuroscience InstituteCornell UniversityIthacaUSA
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyLake Superior State UniversitySault Ste. MarieUSA

Personalised recommendations