Advertisement

On the memory processes underlying conscious deliberation in complex decision making: the role of verbatim and gist memory

  • Marlène AbadieEmail author
  • Laurent Waroquier
Original Article
  • 24 Downloads

Abstract

Studies showed that a distraction period improves complex decision making relative to a conscious deliberation period or an immediate choice. Although this counterintuitive finding was replicated several times, many other studies failed to find any beneficial effect of distraction and some even showed situations in which conscious deliberation was more effective. We suggest that studies showing a conscious thought advantage share several features that may have fostered the encoding and the retrieval of precise verbatim representations of the choice alternatives. The effectiveness of conscious deliberation could thus depend on the availability of verbatim memory. To test this hypothesis, we varied the availability of verbatim memory for the attributes of various equivalent alternatives by introducing, for half of the participants, a time delay between the presentation of the alternatives and of a fictitious client request that provides a normative criterion to evaluate them. Verbatim memory declined, whereas gist memory increased in the delay relative to the no delay condition. Moreover, there was a detrimental effect of delay in the deliberation condition but not in the distraction and the immediate decision conditions. Both verbatim memory and decision quality after deliberation were affected by the introduction of a delay which suggests that verbatim memory underlies conscious thought effectiveness.

Notes

References

  1. Abadie, M., Villejoubert, G., Waroquier, L., & Vallée-Tourangeau, F. (2013). The interplay between presentation material and decision mode for complex choice preferences. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 682–691.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Abadie, M., & Waroquier, L. (2019). Evaluating the benefits of conscious and unconscious thought in complex decision-making. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 72–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Abadie, M., Waroquier, L., & Terrier, P. (2013). Gist memory in the unconscious-thought effect. Psychological Science, 24, 1253–1259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Abadie, M., Waroquier, L., & Terrier, P. (2015). Information presentation format moderates the unconscious-thought effect: The role of recollection. Memory, 24, 1123–1133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Abadie, M., Waroquier, L., & Terrier, P. (2017). The role of gist and verbatim memory in complex decision making: Explaining the unconscious-thought effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43, 694–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Acker, F. (2008). New findings on unconscious versus conscious thought in decision making: Additional empirical data and meta-analysis. Judgment and Decision Making, 3, 292–303.Google Scholar
  7. Ashby, N. J. S., Glöckner, A., & Dickert, S. (2011). Conscious and unconscious thought in risky choice: Testing the capacity principle and the appropriate weighting principle of unconscious thought theory. Frontiers in Psychology.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00261.Google Scholar
  8. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Aydin, C. (2010). Remembering in contradictory minds: disjunction fallacies in episodic memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 711–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brainerd, C. J., Reyna, V. F., & Mojardin, A. H. (1999). Conjoint recognition. Psychological Review, 106, 160–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Damian, R. I., & Sherman, J. W. (2013). A process-dissociation examination of the cognitive processes underlying unconscious thought. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 228–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dijksterhuis, A. (2004). Think different: the merits of unconscious thought in preference development and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 586–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311, 1005–1007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dijksterhuis, A., & Nordgren, L. F. (2006). A theory of unconscious thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 95–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Dijksterhuis, A., & Strick, M. (2016). A case for thinking without consciousness. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 117–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Huizenga, H. M., Wetzels, R., van Ravenzwaaij, D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2012). Four empirical tests of unconscious thought theory. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 117, 332–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Jonides, J., Schumacher, E. H., Smith, E., Lauber, E. J., Awh, E., Minoshima, S., & Koeppe, R. A. (1997). Verbal working memory load affects regional brain activation as measured by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 462–475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Klauer, K. C., Stahl, C., & Voss, A. (2011). Multinomial models and diffusion models. In A. Voss, C. Stahl, & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), Cognitive methods in social psychology (pp. 331–354). La Rioja: Dialnet.Google Scholar
  19. Li, J., Gao, Q., Zhou, J., Li, X., Zhang, M., & Shen, M. (2014). Bias or equality? Unconscious thought equally integrates temporally scattered information. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 77–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Newell, B. R., & Shanks, D. R. (2014). Unconscious influences on decision making: A critical review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 1–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Newell, B. R., Wong, K. Y., Cheung, J. C. H., & Rakow, T. (2008). Think, blink or sleep on it? The impact of modes of thought on complex decision making. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 707–732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Nieuwenstein, M. R., Wierenga, T., Morey, R. D., Wicherts, J. M., Blom, T. N., Wagenmakers, E.-J., & van Rijn, H. (2015). On making the right choice: A meta- analysis and large-scale replication attempt of the unconscious thought advantage. Judgment and Decision Making, 10, 1–17.Google Scholar
  23. Payne, J. W., Samper, A., Bettman, J. R., & Luce, M. F. (2008). Boundary conditions on unconscious thought in complex decision making. Psychological Science, 19, 1118–11123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Reyna, V. F. (2012). A new intuitionism: Meaning, memory, and development in fuzzy-trace theory. Judgment and Decision Making, 7, 332–359.Google Scholar
  25. Reyna, V. F. (2018). When irrational biases are smart: A fuzzy-trace theory of complex decision making. Journal of Intelligence, 6, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Reyna, V. F., & Brainerd, C. J. (1995). Fuzzy-trace theory: An interim synthesis. Learning and Individual Differences, 7, 1–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Reyna, V. F., & Kiernan, B. (1994). Development of gist versus verbatim memory in sentence recognition: Effects of lexical familiarity, semantic content, encoding instructions, and retention interval. Developmental Psychology, 30, 178–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Stahl, C., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). A simplified conjoint recognition paradigm for the measurement of gist and verbatim memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 570–586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Sjoerdma, A., van Baaren, R. B., & Nordgren, L. F. (2011). A meta-analysis on unconscious thought effects. Social Cognition, 29, 738–762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Strick, M., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R. B. (2010). Unconscious-thought effects take place off-line, not on-line. Psychological Science, 21, 484–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Waroquier, L., Abadie, M., Klein, O., & Cleeremans, A. (2014). The effect of the cognitive demands of the distraction task on unconscious thought. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37, 44–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Waroquier, L., Marchiori, D., Klein, O., & Cleeremans, A. (2009). Methodological pitfalls of the unconscious thought paradigm. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 601–610.Google Scholar
  34. Waroquier, L., Marchiori, D., Klein, O., & Cleeremans, A. (2010). Is it better to think unconsciously or to trust your first impression? A reassessment of unconscious thought theory. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 111–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 441–517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie CognitiveAix-Marseille Université, CNRS & AMU, UMR7290Marseille Cedex 3France
  2. 2.Université Clermont Auvergne, CNRS, LAPSCOClermont-FerrandFrance

Personalised recommendations