Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 83, Issue 1, pp 185–195 | Cite as

Do movements contribute to sense of body ownership? Rubber hand illusion in expert pianists

  • Maria PyasikEmail author
  • Adriana Salatino
  • Lorenzo Pia
Original Article

Abstract

Currently, it is still debated whether, how and to what extent movements contribute to the sense of body ownership (i.e., the feeling that one’s body belongs to oneself). To answer this question, here we examined if a prolonged increase of the amount of movements affects body ownership. Specifically, we administered the rubber hand illusion paradigm within a natural condition of long-term motor practice, namely in expert pianists. We compared the illusory effects of both static (visuotactile stimulation) and dynamic (active/passive movements) versions of that paradigm in a group of expert pianists and a group of non-musicians. The illusion was measured behaviorally (proprioceptive drift) and subjectively (questionnaire). Our results showed that pianists were significantly less susceptible to any type of the illusion, compared to the non-musicians. Moreover, they did not experience the illusion in general (presenting neither the proprioceptive drift, nor the subjective feeling of ownership). These findings suggest that the increased amount of motor-related afferent and efferent signals does affect the construction and the coherence of body ownership, thus showing the role of movements in this process.

Notes

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Supplementary material

426_2018_1137_MOESM1_ESM.xlsx (19 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (XLSX 18 KB)

References

  1. Adamo, D. E., Scotland, S., & Martin, B. J. (2012). Upper limb kinesthetic asymmetries: Gender and handedness effects. Neuroscience Letters, 516(2), 188–192.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.077.Google Scholar
  2. Blakemore, S. J., Frith, C. D., & Wolpert, D. M. (1999). Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(5), 551–559.  https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563607.Google Scholar
  3. Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M., & Frith, C. D. (1998). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature Neuroscience, 1(7), 635–640.  https://doi.org/10.1038/2870.Google Scholar
  4. Bottini, G., Sedda, A., Ferre, E. R., Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., & Paulesu, E. (2009). Productive symptoms in right brain damage. Current Opinion in Neurology, 22(6), 589–593.  https://doi.org/10.1097/WCO.0b013e328332c71d.Google Scholar
  5. Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature, 391(6669), 756.  https://doi.org/10.1038/35784.Google Scholar
  6. Burin, D., Garbarini, F., Bruno, V., Fossataro, C., Destefanis, C., Berti, A., & Pia, L. (2017). Movements and body ownership: Evidence from the rubber hand illusion after mechanical limb immobilization. Neuropsychologia, 107, 41–47.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.004.Google Scholar
  7. Burin, D., Livelli, A., Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Folegatti, A., Gindri, P., & Pia, L. (2015). Are movements necessary for the sense of body ownership? Evidence from the rubber hand illusion in pure hemiplegic patients. PLoS One, 10, 3.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117155.
  8. Burin, D., Pyasik, M., Ronga, I., Cavallo, M., Salatino, A., & Pia, L. (2018). As long as that is my hand, that willed action is mine”: Timing of agency triggered by body ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 186–192.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CONCOG.2017.12.005.Google Scholar
  9. Burin, D., Pyasik, M., Salatino, A., & Pia, L. (2017). That’s my hand! Therefore, that’s my willed action: How body ownership acts upon conscious awareness of willed actions. Cognition, 166, 164–173.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.035.Google Scholar
  10. Costantini, M., & Haggard, P. (2007). The rubber hand illusion: Sensitivity and reference frame for body ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(2), 229–240.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.01.001.Google Scholar
  11. Costantini, M., Robinson, J., Migliorati, D., Donno, B., Ferri, F., & Northoff, G. (2016). Temporal limits on rubber hand illusion reflect individuals’ temporal resolution in multisensory perception. Cognition, 157, 39–48.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.010.Google Scholar
  12. David, N., Fiori, F., & Aglioti, S. M. (2014). Susceptibility to the rubber hand illusion does not tell the whole body-awareness story. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 297–306.  https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0190-6.Google Scholar
  13. Dummer, T., Picot-Annand, A., Neal, T., & Moore, C. (2009). Movement and the rubber hand illusion. Perception, 38(2), 271–280.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p5921.Google Scholar
  14. Ehrsson, H. H., Spence, C., & Passingham, R. E. (2004). That’s my hand! Activity in premotor cortex reflects feeling of ownership of a limb. Science, 305(5685), 875–877.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1097011.Google Scholar
  15. Ericsson, K. A. (2014). Why expert performance is special and cannot be extrapolated from studies of performance in the general population: A response to criticisms. Intelligence, 45, 81–103.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.INTELL.2013.12.001.Google Scholar
  16. Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363–406.  https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.100.3.363.Google Scholar
  17. Farnè, A., Pavani, F., Meneghello, F., & Ladavas, E. (2000). Left tactile extinction following visual stimulation of a rubber hand. Brain, 123(11), 2350–2360.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.11.2350.Google Scholar
  18. Fiorio, M., Weise, D., Önal-Hartmann, C., Zeller, D., Tinazzi, M., & Classen, J. (2011). Impairment of the rubber hand illusion in focal hand dystonia. Brain, 134(5), 1428–1437.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr026.Google Scholar
  19. Frith, C. (2005). The self in action: Lessons from delusions of control. Consciousness and Cognition, 14(4), 752–770.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.04.002.Google Scholar
  20. Gallagher, S. (2000). Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences.  https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01417-5.Google Scholar
  21. Gallese, V., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The bodily self as power for action. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 746–755.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.038 Google Scholar
  22. Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9240–9245.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.23-27-09240.2003.Google Scholar
  23. Hartcher-O’Brien, J., Gallace, A., Krings, B., Koppen, C., & Spence, C. (2008). When vision ‘extinguishes’ touch in neurologically-normal people: Extending the Colavita visual dominance effect. Experimental Brain Research, 186(4), 643–658.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1272-5.Google Scholar
  24. Hartcher-O’Brien, J., Levitan, C., & Spence, C. (2010). Extending visual dominance over touch for input off the body. Brain Research, 1362, 48–55.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2010.09.036.Google Scholar
  25. Head, H., & Holmes, J. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34, 102–254.  https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/34.2-3.102.Google Scholar
  26. Hosoda, M., & Furuya, S. (2016). Shared somatosensory and motor functions in musicians. Scientific Reports, 6, 37632.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep37632.Google Scholar
  27. Ismail, M. A. F., & Shimada, S. (2016). “Robot” hand illusion under delayed visual feedback: Relationship between the senses of ownership and agency. PLoS One, 11(7), e0159619.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159619.Google Scholar
  28. James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
  29. Jenkinson, P. M., Haggard, P., Ferreira, N. C., & Fotopoulou, A. (2013). Body ownership and attention in the mirror: Insights from somatoparaphrenia and the rubber hand illusion. Neuropsychologia, 51(8), 1453–1462.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.03.029.Google Scholar
  30. Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: A dissociation of ownership and agency. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 40.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00040.Google Scholar
  31. Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: Comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 26C, 117–132.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.003.Google Scholar
  32. Kammers, M. P. M., de Vignemont, F., Verhagen, L., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2009). The rubber hand illusion in action. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 204–211.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.028.Google Scholar
  33. Kerr, C. E., Agrawal, U., & Nayak, S. (2016). The effects of tai chi practice on intermuscular beta coherence and the rubber hand illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 37.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00037.Google Scholar
  34. Kilteni, K., Maselli, A., Kording, K. P., & Slater, M. (2015). Over my fake body: Body ownership illusions for studying the multisensory basis of own-body perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 141.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00141.Google Scholar
  35. Lehmann, A. C. (1997). The acquisition of expertise in music: Efficiency of deliberate practice as a moderating variable in accounting for sub-expert performance. In I. Deliège & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Perception and cognition of music (pp. 161–187). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  36. Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., & Blanke, O. (2007). Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science, 317(5841), 1096–1099.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1143439 Google Scholar
  37. Lloyd, D. M. (2007). Spatial limits on referred touch to an alien limb may reflect boundaries of visuo-tactile peripersonal space surrounding the hand. Brain and Cognition, 64(1), 104–109.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BANDC.2006.09.013.Google Scholar
  38. Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2009). Sense of agency primes manual motor responses. Perception, 38(1), 69–78.  https://doi.org/10.1068/p6045.Google Scholar
  39. Longo, M. R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M. P. M., Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). What is embodiment? A psychometric approach. Cognition, 107(3), 978–998.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.004.Google Scholar
  40. Luo, C., Guo, Z., Lai, Y., Liao, W., Liu, Q., Kendrick, K. M., … Li, H. (2012). Musical training induces functional plasticity in perceptual and motor networks: Insights from resting-state fMRI. PLoS One, 7(5), e36568.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036568.Google Scholar
  41. Ma, K., & Hommel, B. (2015). The role of agency for perceived ownership in the virtual hand illusion. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 277–288.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.07.008.Google Scholar
  42. Makin, T. R., Holmes, N. P., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2008). On the other hand: Dummy hands and peripersonal space. Behavioural Brain Research.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.041.Google Scholar
  43. Ocklenburg, S., Rüther, N., Peterburs, J., Pinnow, M., & Güntürkün, O. (2011). Laterality in the rubber hand illusion. Laterality, 16(2), 174–187.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500903483515.Google Scholar
  44. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4.Google Scholar
  45. Pia, L., Garbarini, F., Fossataro, C., Burin, D., & Berti, A. (2016). Sensing the body, representing the body: Evidence from a neurologically based delusion of body ownership. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 33(1–2), 112–119.  https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2016.1185404.Google Scholar
  46. Pia, L., Spinazzola, L., Garbarini, F., Bellan, G., Piedimonte, A., Fossataro, C., Berti, A. (2014). Anosognosia for hemianaesthesia: A voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study. Cortex, 61, 158–166.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.006.Google Scholar
  47. Piedimonte, A., Garbarini, F., Pia, L., Mezzanato, T., & Berti, A. (2016). From intention to perception: The case of anosognosia for hemiplegia. Neuropsychologia, 87, 43–53.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.007.Google Scholar
  48. Preston, C. (2013). The role of distance from the body and distance from the real hand in ownership and disownership during the rubber hand illusion. Acta Psychologica, 142(2), 177–183.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ACTPSY.2012.12.005.Google Scholar
  49. Pyasik, M., Burin, D., & Pia, L. (2018). On the relation between body ownership and sense of agency: A link at the level of sensory-related signals. Acta Psychologica, 185, 219–228.  https://doi.org/10.1016/J.ACTPSY.2018.03.001.Google Scholar
  50. Ragert, P., Schmidt, A., Altenmüller, E., & Dinse, H. R. (2004). Superior tactile performance and learning in professional pianists: Evidence for meta-plasticity in musicians. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19(2), 473–478.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0953-816X.2003.03142.x.Google Scholar
  51. Riemer, M., Kleinbohl, D., Holzl, R., & Trojan, J. (2013). Action and perception in the rubber hand illusion. Experimental Brain Research, 229(3), 383–393.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3374-3.Google Scholar
  52. Scandola, M., Tidoni, E., Avesani, R., Brunelli, G., Aglioti, S. M., & Moro, V. (2014). Rubber hand illusion induced by touching the face ipsilaterally to a deprived hand: Evidence for plastic “somatotopic” remapping in tetraplegics. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 404.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00404.Google Scholar
  53. Scandola, M., Tidoni, E., Avesani, R., Brunelli, G., Aglioti, S. M., & Moro, V. (2014). Rubber hand illusion induced by touching the face ipsilaterally to a deprived hand: Evidence for plastic “somatotopic” remapping in tetraplegics. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 404.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00404.Google Scholar
  54. Schulz, M., Ross, B., & Pantev, C. (2003). Evidence for training-induced crossmodal reorganization of cortical functions in trumpet players. Neuroreport, 14(1), 157–161.  https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnr.0000053061.10406.c7.Google Scholar
  55. Shimada, S., Fukuda, K., & Hiraki, K. (2009). Rubber hand illusion under delayed visual feedback. PLoS One, 4, 7.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0006185.Google Scholar
  56. Smit, M., Kooistra, D. I., van der Ham, I. J. M., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2017). Laterality and body ownership: Effect of handedness on experience of the rubber hand illusion. Laterality Asymmetries of Body Brain and Cognition, 22(6), 703–724.  https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2016.1273940.Google Scholar
  57. Tidoni, E., Grisoni, L., Liuzza, M. T., & Aglioti, S. M. (2014). Rubber hand illusion highlights massive visual capture and sensorimotor face-hand remapping in a tetraplegic man. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 32(5), 611–622.  https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-130385.Google Scholar
  58. Tsakiris, M. (2010). My body in the brain: A neurocognitive model of body-ownership. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 703–712.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.034 Google Scholar
  59. Tsakiris, M., Prabhu, G., & Haggard, P. (2006). Having a body versus moving your body: How agency structures body-ownership. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 423–432.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2005.09.004.Google Scholar
  60. Vallar, G., & Ronchi, R. (2009). Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3), 533–551.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1562-y.Google Scholar
  61. Vaquero, L., Hartmann, K., Ripollés, P., Rojo, N., Sierpowska, J., François, C., Altenmüller, E. (2016). Structural neuroplasticity in expert pianists depends on the age of musical training onset. NeuroImage, 126, 106–119.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.008.Google Scholar
  62. Walsh, L. D., Moseley, G. L., Taylor, J. L., & Gandevia, S. C. (2011). Proprioceptive signals contribute to the sense of body ownership. Journal of Physiology, 589(Pt 12), 3009–3021.  https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2011.204941.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.SAMBA-SpAtial, Motor and Bodily Awareness Research Group, Department of PsychologyUniversity of TurinTurinItaly
  2. 2.NIT-Neuroscience Institute of TurinTurinItaly

Personalised recommendations