Advertisement

The tendency for experiencing involuntary future and past mental time travel is robustly related to thought suppression: an exploratory study

  • Adriana del Palacio-GonzalezEmail author
  • Dorthe Berntsen
Original Article

Abstract

Involuntary mental time travel (MTT) refers to projecting oneself into the past or into the future without prior conscious effort. The previous studies have shown high inter-individual variability in the frequency of involuntary MTT, but a few systematic studies exist. In three exploratory studies, we investigated the relation between individual differences in experiencing involuntary past and future MTT, and selected emotional and cognitive processes, with a special focus on thought suppression. Across all three studies, thought suppression emerged as a robust predictor of involuntary MTT above and beyond emotion-related variables, mind-wandering, daydreaming styles, and demographic variables. Findings from Studies 1 and 2 showed that higher thought suppression consistently predicted both more frequent involuntary past and future MTT across an American and a Danish sample, whereas rumination and emotion regulation were less consistently related to involuntary MTT. In Study 3, thought suppression reliably predicted more frequent involuntary MTT, even when controlling for mind-wandering, as well as for positive and negative daydreaming styles, which were all related to greater involuntary MTT. Overall, the individual differences assessed showed similar relationships to the tendency for having past and future involuntary MTT, with the possible exception of daydreaming styles, which appeared more strongly related to future-directed involuntary MTT.

Notes

Funding

This work was supported by the Danish National Research Foundation (DNRF) under Grant DNRF89. The DNRF had no involvement in the design of the current study or the interpretation of the results.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The author declares that there is no competing interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

References

  1. Aldao, A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2010). Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(10), 974–983.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Alle, M., Berna, F., & Berntsen, D. (2018). Involuntary autobiographical memory and future thought predicting hallucination proneness. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(6), 891–898.Google Scholar
  3. Antrobus, J. S., Singer, J. L., Goldstein, S., & Fortgang, M. (1970). Mind-wandering and cognitive structure. Transactions of the New York Academy of SCIence (Series 11), 32, 242–252.Google Scholar
  4. Beaty, R. E., Seli, P., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). Thinking about the past and future in daily life: an experience sampling study of individual differences in mental time travel. Psychological Research.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1075-7.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Berntsen, D. (1996). Involuntary autobiographical memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10(5), 435–454.Google Scholar
  6. Berntsen, D. (2009). Involuntary autobiographical memories: An introduction to the unbidden past. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Berntsen, D. (2018). Spontaneous future cognitions: A theoretical review, Psychological Research.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1127-z PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Berntsen, D., & Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory and Cognition, 38(3), 265–278.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Berntsen, D., & Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 17(4), 1093–1104.Google Scholar
  10. Berntsen, D., Rubin, D. C., & Salgado, S. (2015). The frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and future thoughts in relation to daydreaming, emotional distress, and age. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 36, 352–372.Google Scholar
  11. Buhrmester, M. D., Talaifar, S., & Gosling, S. D. (2018). An evaluation of Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, its rapid rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 149–154.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. NeurosCIence, Nature Reviews 17, 718–731.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Clark, D. A., & Beck, A. T. (2010). Cognitive therapy of anxiety disorders: SCIence and practice. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  14. Cole, S. N., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Do future thoughts reflect personal goals? Current concerns and mental time travel into the past and future. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 273–284.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Cole, S. N., Staugaard, S., & Berntsen, D. (2016). Inducing involuntary and voluntary mental time travel using a laboratory paradigm. Memory and Cognition, 44(3), 376–389.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12(11), 671–684.Google Scholar
  17. Cummings, G. (2009). Inference by eye: Reading the overlap of independent confidence intervals. Statistics in MediCIne, 28, 205–220.Google Scholar
  18. D’Argembeau, A. (2012). Autobiographical memory and future thinking. In D. Berntsen & D. C. Rubin (Eds.). Understanding autobiographical memory: Theories and approaches (pp. 311–330). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Lindeen, M. (2006). Individual differences in the phenomenology of mental time travel: The effect of vivid visual imagery and emotion regulation strategies. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 15, 342–350.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018a). Emotional responses to everyday mental time travel in social anxiety: A naturalistic study. (Manuscript in preparation).Google Scholar
  21. del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., & Berntsen, D. (2018b). Emotion regulation of events central to identity and their relationship with concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms. Behavior Therapy, 49, 604–616.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. del Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Berntsen, D., & Watson, L. A. (2017). Emotional intensity and emotion regulation in response to autobiographical memories during dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(4), 530–542.Google Scholar
  23. Erskine, J. A. K., Kvavilashvili, L., & Kornbrot, D. E. (2007). The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: Effects of rumination, anxiety, and other variables. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(6), 1047–1057.Google Scholar
  24. Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2011). Involuntary and voluntary mental time travel in high and low worriers. Memory, 19(6), 625–640.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Finnbogadóttir, H., & Berntsen, D. (2013). Involuntary future projections are as frequent as involuntary memories, but more positive. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 22(1), 272–280.Google Scholar
  26. Giambra, L. M. (1993). The influence of aging on spontaneous shifts of attention from external stimuli to the contents of consciousness. Experimental Gerontology, 28, 485–492.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Grysman, A. (2015). Collecting narrative data on amazon’s mechanical turk. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(4), 573–583.Google Scholar
  29. Hintzman, D. L. (2011). Research strategy in the study of memory: Fads, fallacies, and the search for the “coordinates of truth”. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 6(3), 253–271.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Huba, G. J., Segal, B., & Singer, J. L. (1977). Consistency of daydreaming styles across samples of college male and female drug and alcohol users. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(1), 99–102.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Huba, G. J., Singer, J. L., Aneshenel, C. S., & Antrobus, J. S. (1982). Short Imaginal Process Inventory. In Short Imaginal Porcesses Inventory. Manual. London: Research Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  32. Hyman, I. E. Jr., Cutshaw, K. I., Hall, C. M., Snyders, M. E., Masters, S. A., Au, V. S. K., & Graham, J. M. (2015). Involuntary to intrusive: Using involuntary musical imagery to explore individual differences and the nature of intrusive thoughts. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 25(1), 14–27.Google Scholar
  33. Jones, S. R., & Fernyhough, C. (2009). Rumination, reflection, intrusive thoughts, and hallucination-proneness: Towards a new model. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(1), 54–59.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Kamiya, S. (2014). Relationship between frequency of involuntary autobiographical memories and cognitive failure. Memory, 22(7), 839–851.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Koster, E. H. W., Soetens, B., Braet, C., & De Raedt, R. (2008). How to control a white bear? Individual differences involved in self-perceived and actual thought-suppression ability. Cognition and Emotion, 22(6), 1068–1080.Google Scholar
  36. Krans, J., de Bree, J., & Moulds, M. L. (2015). Involuntary cognitions in everyday life: Exploration of type, quality, content, and function. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 6, 11.Google Scholar
  37. Lavy, E. H., & Van, dH. (1990). Thought suppression induces intrusions. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18(4), 251–258.Google Scholar
  38. Mace, J. H. (2007). Involuntary memory: Concept and theory. In J. H. Mace (Ed.), Involuntary memory; involuntary memory (pp. 1–19). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
  39. Marchetti, I., Koster, E. H. W., Klinger, E., & Alloy, L. B. (2016). Spontaneous thought and vulnerability to mood disorders: The dark side of the wandering mind. Clinical Psychological SCIence, 4(5), 835–857.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  40. McMillan, R. L., Kaufman, S. B., & Singer, J. L. (2013). Ode to positive constructive daydreaming. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–9.Google Scholar
  41. Meyer, T., Otgaar, H., & Smeets, T. (2015). Flashbacks, intrusions, mind-wandering - instances of an involuntary memory spectrum: A commentary on Takarangi, Strange, and Lindsay (2014). ConsCIousness and Cognition, 33, 24–29.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Miles, L. K., Karpinska, K., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2010). The meandering mind: Vection and mental time travel. PLoS One, 5(5), e10825.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  43. Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the mind-wandering questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 7.Google Scholar
  44. Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Horselenberg, R. (1996). Individual differences in thought suppression. the white bear suppression inventory: Factor structure, reliability, validity and correlates. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(5–6), 501.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Larson, J., & Grayson, C. (1999). Explaining the gender difference in depressive symptoms. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 77(5), 1061–1072.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Palacio-Gonzalez, A., Clark, D. A., & O’Sullivan, L.,F. (2017). Cognitive processing in the aftermath of relationship dissolution: Associations with concurrent and prospective distress and posttraumatic growth. Stress and Health, 33(5), 540–548.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Plimpton, B., Patel, P., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2015). Role of triggers and dysphoria in mind-wandering about past, present and future: A laboratory study. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 33, 261–276.Google Scholar
  48. Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2011). The unpredictable past: Spontaneous autobiographical memories outnumber autobiographical memories retrieved strategically. ConsCIousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 20(4), 1842–1846.Google Scholar
  49. Rasmussen, A. S., & Berntsen, D. (2013). The reality of the past versus the ideality of the future: Emotional valence and functional differences between past and future mental time travel. Memory & Cognition, 41(2), 187–200.Google Scholar
  50. Rasmussen, A. S., Ramsgaard, S. B., & Berntsen, D. (2015). Frequency and functions of involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memories across the day. Psychology of ConsCIousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2(2), 185–205.Google Scholar
  51. Rubin, D. C., Dennis, M. F., & Beckham, J. C. (2011). Autobiographical memory for stressful events: The role of autobiographical memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. ConsCIousness and Cognition, 20(3), 840–856.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Salkovskis, P. M., & Campbell, P. (1994). Thought suppression induces intrusion in naturally occurring negative intrusive thoughts. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(1), 1.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Salkovskis, P. M., & Reynolds, M. (1994). Thought suppression and smoking cessation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(2), 193.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  54. Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the past to imagine the future: The prospective brain. Nature Reviews NeurosCIence, 8(9), 657–661.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Schank, R. C. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Seli, P., Risko, E. F., Smilek, D., & Schacter, D. L. (2016). Mind-wandering with and without intention. Trends in Cognitive SCIences, 20(8), 605–617.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Singer, J. L. (1974). Daydreaming and the stream of thought. American SCIentist, 62(4), 417–425.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the stream of consciousness: Research in daydreaming and related inner experience. American Psychologist, 30(7), 727–738.Google Scholar
  59. Smallwood, J., & Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not all minds that wander are lost: The importance of a balanced perspective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 6.Google Scholar
  60. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487–518.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  62. Smets, J., Wessel, I., Schreurs, E., & Raes, F. (2012). The interplay between rumination and intrusions in the prediction of concurrent and prospective depressive symptoms in two nonclinical samples. The Psychological Record, 62(4), 777–788.Google Scholar
  63. Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic future thought: An emerging concept. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence, 5(2), 142–162.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  64. Thompson, E. R. (2007). Development and validation of an internationally reliable short-form of the positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS). Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(2), 227–242.Google Scholar
  65. Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 247–259.Google Scholar
  66. Turk, I. R. Rise, and its effective use. Perspectives on Psychological SCIence. 13, 149–154.Google Scholar
  67. Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 163–206.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  68. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., I.,II, & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and SoCIal Psychology, 53(1), 5.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of Personality, 62(4), 615.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology and Behavioural Sciences, Center on Autobiographical Memory ResearchAarhus UniversityArhus CDenmark

Personalised recommendations