Advertisement

SNARC-like compatibility effects for physical and phenomenal magnitudes: a study on visual illusions

  • Valter Prpic
  • Alessandro Soranzo
  • Ilaria Santoro
  • Carlo Fantoni
  • Alessandra Galmonte
  • Tiziano Agostini
  • Mauro Murgia
Original Article
  • 19 Downloads

Abstract

Both numerical and non-numerical magnitudes elicit similar Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) effects, with small magnitudes associated with left hand responses and large magnitudes associated with right hand responses (Dehaene et al., J Exp Psychol Gen 122(3), 371, 1993). In the present study, we investigated whether the phenomenal size of visual illusions elicits the same SNARC-like effect revealed for the physical size of pictorial surfaces. Four experiments were conducted by using the Delboeuf illusion (Experiment 1) and the Kanizsa triangle illusion (Experiments 2, 3 and 4). Experiment 1 suggests the presence of a SNARC-like compatibility effect for the physical size of the inducers, while this effect was not revealed for the phenomenal size of the induced elements, possibly masked by a stronger effect of the inducers. A SNARC-like effect for the phenomenal size of the Kanizsa triangle was revealed when participants directly compared the size of the triangles (Experiment 4). Conversely, when participants performed an indirect task (orientation judgment), the SNARC-like effect was present neither for the illusory nor for the physical displays (Experiments 2 and 3). The effect revealed for the size of illusory triangles was comparable to that of real triangles with physical contours, suggesting that both phenomenal and physical magnitudes similarly elicit SNARC-like effects.

Notes

Acknowledgements

A special thanks goes to Mattia Sarcetta, Katie Elliot, Stefania Caporal and Fulvio Bullo for recruiting participants for this study. We also thank Michele Stefanuto and Francesco Sinicco for their help on stimuli creation, and Courtney Goodridge for the English proofreading. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Ethical standards

The present study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the University of Trieste in compliance with national legislation, the Ethical Code of the Italian Association of Psychology, and the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

References

  1. Aglioti, S., DeSouza, J. F., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5(6), 679–685.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bueti, D., & Walsh, V. (2009). The parietal cortex and the representation of time, space, number and other magnitudes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 1831–1840.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Coren, S., Porac, C., & Theodor, L. H. (1986). The effects of perceptual set on the shape and apparent depth of subjective contours. Perception & Psychophysics, 39(5), 327–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Cohen, L. (1998). Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends in Neurosciences, 21(8), 355–361.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(3), 626.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Delboeuf, J. (1865). Seconde note sur de nouvelles illusions d’optique: Essai d’une théorie psychophysique de la manière dont l’oeil apprécie les grandeurs. Bulletins de l’Académie Royale des Sciences, lettres et Beaux-arts de Belgique, 20, 70–97.Google Scholar
  8. Ebbinghaus, H. (1902). Grundzüge der Psychologie, Vol. 1. Leipzig: Veit.Google Scholar
  9. Fias, W., Brysbaert, M., Geypens, F., & d’Ydewalle, G. (1996). The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence from the SNARC effect. Mathematical Cognition, 2(1), 95–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fias, W., Lammertyn, J., Reynvoet, B., Dupont, P., & Orban, G. A. (2003). Parietal representation of symbolic and nonsymbolic magnitude. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 47–56.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Fias, W., Lauwereyns, J., & Lammertyn, J. (2001). Irrelevant digits affect feature-based attention depending on the overlap of neural circuits. Cognitive Brain Research, 12(3), 415–423.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Franz, V. H. (2001). Action does not resist visual illusions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(11), 457–459.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Franz, V. H., & Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Grasping visual illusions: Consistent data and no dissociation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25(7–8), 920–950.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Franz, V. H., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Bülthoff, H. H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11(1), 20–25.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Fumarola, A., Prpic, V., Fornasier, D., Sartoretto, F., Agostini, T., & Umiltà, C. (2016). The spatial representation of Angles. Perception, 45(11), 1320–1330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Fumarola, A., Prpic, V., Pos, D., Murgia, O., Umiltà, M.C., & Agostini, T. (2014). Automatic spatial association for luminance. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76(3), 759–765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Ganel, T., Tanzer, M., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). A double dissociation between action and perception in the context of visual illusions: Opposite effects of real and illusory size. Psychological Science, 19(3), 221–225.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Gevers, W., Verguts, T., Reynvoet, B., Caessens, B., & Fias, W. (2006). Numbers and space: A computational model of the SNARC effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 32.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Goodale, M. A., Meenan, J. P., Bülthoff, H. H., Nicolle, D. A., Murphy, K. J., & Racicot, C. I. (1994). Separate neural pathways for the visual analysis of object shape in perception and prehension. Current Biology, 4(7), 604–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goodale, M. A., Milner, A. D., Jakobson, L. S., & Carey, D. P. (1991). A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature, 349(6305), 154–156.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Haffenden, A. M., Schiff, K. C., & Goodale, M. A. (2001). The dissociation between perception and action in the Ebbinghaus illusion: Nonillusory effects of pictorial cues on grasp. Current Biology, 11(3), 177–181.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Hartmann, M., & Mast, F. W. (2017). Loudness counts: Interactions between loudness, number magnitude, and space. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(7), 1305–1322.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kanizsa, G. (1955). Margini quasi-percettivi in campi con stimolazione omogenea. Rivista di psicologia, 49(1), 7–30.Google Scholar
  24. Kaufman, L., & Rock, I. (1962). The moon illusion. Scientific American, 207(1), 120–131.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Koffka, K. (1963). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.Google Scholar
  26. Lidji, P., Kolinsky, R., Lochy, A., & Morais, J. (2007). Spatial associations for musical stimuli: A piano in the head? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(5), 1189.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Lorch, R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(1), 149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Macnamara, A., Keage, H. A., & Loetscher, T. (2018). Mapping of non-numerical domains on space: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Experimental Brain Research, 236, 335–346.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-017-5154-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Meyer, G. E., & Petry, S. (1987). Top-down and bottom-up: The illusory contour as a microcosm of issues in perception. In S. Petry & G. E. Meyer (Eds.), The perception of illusory contours (pp. 3–20). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Mitchell, T., Bull, R., & Cleland, A. A. (2012). Implicit response-irrelevant number information triggers the SNARC effect: Evidence using a neural overlap paradigm. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1945–1961.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Nuerk, H. C., Wood, G., & Willmes, K. (2005). The universal SNARC effect: The association between number magnitude and space is amodal. Experimental Psychology, 52(3), 187.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Piazza, M., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D., & Dehaene, S. (2007). A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron, 53(2), 293–305.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Proctor, R. W., & Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 416.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Prpic, V., & Domijan, D. (2018). Linear representation of pitch height in the SMARC effect. Psychological Topics (in press).Google Scholar
  35. Prpic, V., Fumarola, A., De Tommaso, M., Baldassi, G., & Agostini, T. (2013). A SNARC-like effect for music tempo. Review of Psychology, 20(1–2), 47–51.Google Scholar
  36. Prpic, V., Fumarola, A., De Tommaso, M., Luccio, R., Murgia, M., & Agostini, T. (2016). Separate Mechanisms for Magnitude and Order Processing in the Spatial-Numerical Association of Response Codes (SNARC) Effect: The Strange Case of Musical Note Values. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42(8), 1241.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Ren, P., Nicholls, M. E., Ma, Y., & Chen, L. (2011). Size matters: Non-numerical magnitude affects the spatial coding of response. PLoS One, 6(8), e23553.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  38. Restle, F. (1970). Speed of adding and comparing numbers. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 95, 437–444.Google Scholar
  39. Rock, I., & Kaufman, L. (1962). The moon illusion, II: The moon’s apparent size is a function of the presence or absence of terrain. Science, 136(3521), 1023–1031.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Rusconi, E., Kwan, B., Giordano, B. L., Umilta, C., & Butterworth, B. (2006). Spatial representation of pitch height: The SMARC effect. Cognition, 99(2), 113–129.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Smeets, J. B., & Brenner, E. (2006). 10 years of illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1501.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Titchener, E. B. (1905). Experimental psychology: A manual of laboratory practice (Vol. 2). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  43. Van Opstal, F., & Verguts, T. (2013). Is there a generalized magnitude system in the brain? Behavioral, neuroimaging, and computational evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 435.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Wood, G., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H. C., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). On the cognitive link between space and number: A meta-analysis of the SNARC effect. Psychology Science, 50(4), 489–525.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Institue for Psychological Science, Faculty of Health and Life SciencesDe Montfort UniversityLeicesterUK
  2. 2.Department of Psychology, Sociology and PoliticsSheffield Hallam UniversitySheffieldUK
  3. 3.Department of Life SciencesUniversity of TriesteTriesteItaly
  4. 4.Department of Medical, Surgical and Health SciencesUniversity of TriesteTriesteItaly

Personalised recommendations