Advertisement

Differences in modal distortion in time perception due to working memory capacity: a response with a developmental study in children and adults

  • Sylvie Droit-Volet
  • Quentin Hallez
Original Article

Abstract

This study tested the modality effect on time judgment in a bisection task in children and adults when auditory and visual stimuli were presented in the same session. Cognitive capacities of children and adults were assessed with different neuropsychological tests. The results showed a modality effect, with the auditory stimuli judged longer than the visual stimuli. However, this modality distortion in time judgment was higher in the younger children. Statistical analyses revealed that the size of this time distortion was directly related to individual working memory capacities.

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Sophie Biron (Psychology Master’s student co-directed by a PhD student, Pierre Zélanti) who collected a large proportion (73%) of the children’s data at school, the other children being recruited by Quentin Hallez (PhD student). We also thank Laétitia Bartomeuf (research assistant) who collected the adults’ data at the university. We are also grateful to the directors and the teachers of the Elsa Triolet nursery school at Vic-Le-Conte and the “Centre” primary school at Issoire.

Funding

This study was funded by H2020 European Research Council (BE) (TIMESTORM—H2020—FETPROACT-2014).

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

Sylvie Droit-Volet declares that she has no conflict of interest. Quentin Hallez declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

This experiment was conducted in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki declaration. All children were voluntary to participate in this study. The children’s parents, and the students signed written informed consent to participate in this study, which was approved by the ethical standards of the French research committee (academy) of the French National Education Ministry.

References

  1. Asaoka, R., & Gyoba, J. (2015). Effects of sensory modality and retention delay on time reproduction performance. The Japanese Journal of Psychonomic Science, 34(1), 53–59.Google Scholar
  2. Atkinson, J. (2000). The developing visual brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 417–422.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47–90). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bjorklund, D. V., & Causey, K. B. (2018). Children’s thinking: Cognitive development and individual differences. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publication.Google Scholar
  6. Bueti, D. (2011). The sensory representation of time. Frontiers in integrative neurosciences, 5(34), 1–3.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2011.00034.Google Scholar
  7. Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005). Imaging the developing brain: What have learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(3), 104–110.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Chen, K.-M., & Yeh, S.-L. (2009). Asymmetric cross-modal effects in time perception. Acta Psychologica, 130, 225–234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Church, R. M. (1984). Properties of the internal clock. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 566–582.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Cohen, M. J. (1997). Examiner manual: Children memory scale. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  11. Cowan, N., Wood, N. L., Wood, P. K., Keller, T. A., Nugent, L. D., & Keller, C. V. (1998). Two separate verbal processing speeds contributing to verbal short-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 141–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Creelman, C. D. (1963). Human discrimination of auditory duration. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 34, 582–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Droit-Volet, S. (2013). Time perception in children: A neurodevelopmental approach. Neuropsychologia, 51, 220–234.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Droit-Volet, S. (2016). Development of time. Current Opinion in Behavioural Sciences, 8, 102–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Droit-Volet, S. (2017). Time dilation in children and adults: the idea of a slower internal clock in young children tested with different click frequencies. Behavioural Processes, 138, 152–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Droit-Volet, S., & Coull, J. (2016). Distinct developmental trajectories for explicit and implicit timing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 141–154.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Droit-Volet, S., Meck, W., & Penney, T. (2007). Sensory modality effect and time perception in children and adults. Behavioural Processes, 74, 244–250.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Droit-Volet, S., Tourret, S., & Wearden, J. (2004). Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 5, 797–818.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Droit-Volet, S., & Zélanti, P. (2013). Time sensitivity in children and adults: duration ratios in bisection. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(4), 687–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Droit-Volet, S., & Zélanti, P. S. (2013). Development of time sensitivity and information processing speed. PLoS One, 8(8), e71424.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The Structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 2, 177–190.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279–325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gibbon, J., Church, R. M., & Meck, W. H. (1984). Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 52–77.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Goldstone, S., & Goldfarb, J. (1964a). Auditory and visual time judgement. The Journal of General Psychology, 70, 369–387.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Goldstone, S., & Goldfarb, J. (1964b). Direct comparison of auditory and visual durations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 483–485.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Goldstone, S., & Lhamon, W. T. (1972). Auditory-visual differences in human temporal judgement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 34, 623–633.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Goldstone, S., & Lhamon, W. T. (1974). Studies of auditory-visual differences in human timing judgment, 1: Sounds are judged longer than lights. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 39, 63–82.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Grondin, S., Meilleur-Wells, G., Ouellette, C., & Macar, F. (1998). Sensory effects on judgements of short-time intervals. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 61, 261–268.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Gu, B.-M., van Rijn, H., & Meck, W. H. (2015). Oscillatory multiplexing of neural population codes for interval timing and working memory. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 48, 160–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hallez, Q., & Droit-Volet, S. (2017). High levels of time contraction in young children in dual task are related to their limited attention capacities. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 161, 148–160Google Scholar
  31. Hart, H., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., & Rubia, K. (2012). Meta-analysis of fMRI studies of timing in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2248–2256.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Jain, A., Bansal, R., Kumar, A., & Singh, K. D. (2015). comparative study of visual and auditory reaction times on the basis of gender and physical activity levels of medical first year students. International Journal of Applied and Basic Medical Research, 5(2), 124–127.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Johnstone, B., Erdal, K., & Stadler, M. A. (1995). The relationship between the wechsler memory scale-revised (WMS-R) attention index and putative measures of attention. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 2(2), 195–204.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Litovsky, R. (2015). Development of the auditory system. Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 129, 55–72.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Lustig, C., & Meck, W. H. (2011). Modality difference in timing and temporal memory throughout the lifespan. Brain and Cognition, 77, 298–303.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1999). Tests of everyday attention for children. Flempton: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
  37. McCormack, T. (2015). The development of temporal cognition. In: R. M. Lerner, L. S. Liben & U. Müller, Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, Cognitive Processes, 7th edition, Volume 2. Willey, pp. 624–670.Google Scholar
  38. Ogden, R. S., Salominaite, E., Jones, L. A., Fisk, J. E., & Montgomery, C. (2011). The role of executive functions in human prospective interval timing. Acta Psychologica, 137, 352–358.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Ogden, R. S., Samuels, M., Simmons, F., Wearden, J., & Montgomery, C. (2017). The differential recruitment of short-term memory and executive functions during time, number, and length perception: An individual differences approach. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1–14.Google Scholar
  40. Ogden, R. S., Wearden, J. H., & Montgomery, C. (2014). The differential contribution of executive functions to temporal generalization, reproduction and verbal estimation. Acta Psychologica, 152, 84–94.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Ortega, L., Lopez, F., & Church, R. M. (2009). Modality and intermittency effects on time estimation. Behavioural Processes, 81, 270–273.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Penney, T. B. (2003). Modality differences in interval timing: Attention, clock speed, and memory. In W. H. Meck (Ed.), Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing (pp. 209–234). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Google Scholar
  43. Penney, T. B., Gibbon, J., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1770–1787.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Penney, T. B., & Tourret, S. (2005). Les effets de la modalité sensorielle sur la perception du temps. Psychologie Française, 5(1), 131–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rammsayer, T. H., Borter, N., & Troche, S. J. (2015). Visual-auditory differences in duration discrimination of intervals in the subsecond and second range. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 1626.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01626.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Recanzone, G. H. (2009). Interactions of auditory and visual stimuli in space and time. Hear Res, 258(1–2), 89–99.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  47. Rousseau, L., & Rousseau, R. (1996). Stop-reaction time and the internal clock. Perception and Psychophysics, 58, 434–448.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Roux, F., & Uhlaas, P. J. (2014). Working memory and neural oscillations: α-γ versus θ-γ codes for distinct WM information? Trends in cognitive sciences, 18(1), 16–25.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Rustic, J., & Enns, J. T. (2015). Attentional development. In R. M. Lerner, L. Liben & U. Mueller (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (pp. 158–203). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  50. Sowell, E. R., Delis, D., Stiles, J., & Jernigan, T. L. (2001). Improved memory functioning and frontal lobe maturation between childhood and adolescence: A structural MRI study. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 312–322.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Sowell, E. R., Peterson, B. S., Thompson, P. M., Welcome, S. E., Henkenius, A. L., & Toga, A. W. (2003). Mapping cortical change across the human life span. Nature Neuroscience, 6(3), 309–315.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Stauffer, C. C., Haldemann, J., Troche, S. J., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2012). Auditory and visual temporal sensitivity: evidence for a hierarchical structure of modality-specific and modality-independent levels of temporal information processing. Psychological Research, 76, 20–31.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the” internal clock”. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 77(13), 1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Truett, A., Hume, A. L., Wood, C., & Goff, W. R. L. (1984). Developmental and aging changes in somatosensory, auditory and visual evoked potentials. Electroencephalography and clinical Neurophysiology, 58, 14–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Tsujimoto, S. (2008). The prefrontal cortex: Functional neural development during early childhood. The Neuroscientist, 14(4), 345–358.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Walker, J., & Scott, K. (1981). Auditory-visual conflicts in the perceived duration of lights, tones, and gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 1327–1339.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Wearden, J. (2016). The psychology of time perception. London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wearden, J., Edwards, H., Fakhri, M., & Percival, A. (1998). Why sounds are judged longer than lights: Application of a model of the internal clock in humans. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51B, 97–120.Google Scholar
  59. Wearden, J., Todd, N., & Jones, L. (2006). When do auditory-visual difference in duration judgment occur. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1709–1724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wechsler, D. (1998). WAIS III and WMS-III manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  61. Zélanti, P., & Droit-Volet, S. (2011). Cognitive abilities explaining age-related changes in time perception of short and long durations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(2), 143–157.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Zélanti, P., & Droit-Volet, S. (2012). Auditory and visual differences in time perception? An investigation from a developmental perspective with neuropsychological tests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 296–311.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Université Clermont Auvergne, Laboratoire de psychologie sociale et cognitive, CNRS, UMR 6024Clermont-FerrandFrance

Personalised recommendations