No evidence of task co-representation in a joint Stroop task

  • Daniel R. Saunders
  • David Melcher
  • Wieske van Zoest
Original Article

Abstract

People working together on a task must often represent the goals and salient items of their partner. The aim of the present study was to study the influence of joint task representations in an interference task in which the congruency relies on semantic identity. If task representations are shared between partners in a joint Stroop task (co-representation account), we hypothesized that items in the response set of one partner might influence performance of the other. In Experiment 1, pairs of participants sat side by side. Each participant was instructed to press one of two buttons to indicate which of two colors assigned to them was present, ignoring the text and responding only to the pixel color. There were three types of incongruent distractor words: names of colors from their own response set, names of colors from the other partner’s response set, and neutral words for colors not used as font colors. The results of Experiment 1 showed that when people were doing this task together, distractor words from the partner’s response set interfered more than neutral words and just as much as the words from their own response color set. However, in three follow-up experiments (Experiments 2a, 2b, and 2c), we found an elevated interference for the other response-set words even though no co-actor was present. The overall pattern of results across our study suggests that an alternative response set, regardless of whether it belonged to a co-actor or to a non-social no-go condition, evoked equal amounts of interference comparable to those of the own response set. Our findings are in line with a theory of common coding, in which all events—irrespective of their social nature—are represented and can influence behavior.

References

  1. Atmaca, S., Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2011). The joint flanker effect: sharing tasks with real and imagined co-actors. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3–4), 371–385. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2709-9.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Augustinova, M., & Ferrand, L. (2012). The influence of mere social presence on Stroop interference: new evidence from the semantically-based Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(5), 1213–1216. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.04.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Böckler, A., Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2012). Effects of a coactor’s focus of attention on task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(6), 1404–1415. doi:10.1037/a0027523.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An Analysis of Transformations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 26(2), 211–252.Google Scholar
  5. Demiral, Ş. B., Gambi, C., Nieuwland, M. S., & Pickering, M. J. (2016). Neural correlates of verbal joint action: ERPs reveal common perception and action systems in a shared-Stroop task. Brain Research, 1649((Pt A)), 79–89. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2016.08.025.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Dittrich, K., Bossert, M.-L., Rothe-Wulf, A., & Klauer, K. C. (2017). The joint flanker effect and the joint Simon effect: on the comparability of processes underlying joint compatibility effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(9), 1808–1823. doi:10.1080/17470218.2016.1207690.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Dittrich, K., Dolk, T., Rothe-Wulf, A., Klauer, K. C., & Prinz, W. (2013). Keys and seats: spatial response coding underlying the joint spatial compatibility effect. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(8), 1725–1736. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0524-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dittrich, K., Rothe, A., & Klauer, K. C. (2012). Increased spatial salience in the social Simon task: a response-coding account of spatial compatibility effects. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 74(5), 911–929. doi:10.3758/s13414-012-0304-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schutz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014a). The joint Simon effect a review and theoretical integration. Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/Fpsyg.2014.00974.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., Schütz-Bosbach, S., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2011). How “Social” is the social Simon effect? Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00084.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2013). The (not so) social simon effect: a referential coding account. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1248–1260. doi:10.1037/a0031031.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Dolk, T., Hommel, B., Prinz, W., & Liepelt, R. (2014b). The joint flanker effect: less social than previously thought. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(5), 1224–1230. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0583-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 380–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frischen, A., Loach, D., & Tipper, S. P. (2009). Seeing the world through another person’s eyes: simulating selective attention via action observation. Cognition, 111(2), 212–218. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Guerin, B. (1983). Social facilitation and social monitoring: a test of three models. British Journal of Social Psychology, 22(3), 203–214. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1983.tb00585.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research, 73(4), 512–526. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0234-2.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Hommel, B., Colzato, L. S., & van den Wildenberg, W. P. M. (2009). How Social Are Task Representations? Psychological Science, 20(7), 794–798. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02367.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878. (discussion 878-937).CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Huguet, P., Dumas, F., & Monteil, J.-M. (2004). Competing for a desired reward in the stroop task: when attentional control is unconscious but effective versus conscious but ineffective. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 58(3), 153–167. doi:10.1037/h0087441.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Huguet, P., Galvaing, M. P., Monteil, J. M., & Dumas, F. (1999). Social presence effects in the Stroop task: further evidence for an attentional view of social facilitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(5), 1011.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Klein, G. S. (1964). Semantic power measured through the interference of words with color-naming. The American Journal of Psychology, 77(4), 576–588. doi:10.2307/1420768.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Knoblich, G., Butterfill, S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Psychological research on joint action: theory and data. Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, 54(54), 59–101. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-385527-5.00003-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kornblum, S., Hasbroucq, T., & Osman, A. (1990). Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility—a model and taxonomy. Psychological Review, 97(2), 253–270.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Krebs, R. M., Boehler, C. N., & Woldorff, M. G. (2010). The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task. Cognition, 117(3), 341–347. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.018.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Lamers, M. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Rabeling-Keus, I. M. (2010). Selective attention and response set in the Stroop task. Memory & Cognition, 38(7), 893–904. doi:10.3758/MC.38.7.893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Liefooghe, B. (2016). Joint task switching. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 28(1), 60–78. doi:10.1080/20445911.2015.1084311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Lien, M.-C., Pedersen, L., & Proctor, R. W. (2016). Stimulus-response correspondence in go-nogo and choice tasks: are reactions altered by the presence of an irrelevant salient object? Psychological Research, 80(6), 912–934. doi:10.1007/s00426-015-0699-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Liepelt, R., Wenke, D., Fischer, R., & Prinz, W. (2011). Trial-to-trial sequential dependencies in a social and non-social Simon task. Psychological Research, 75(5), 366–375. doi:10.1007/s00426-010-0314-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Memelink, J., & Hommel, B. (2013). Intentional weighting: a basic principle in cognitive control. Psychological Research, 77(3), 249–259. doi:10.1007/s00426-012-0435-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Müller, B. C. N., Brass, M., Kühn, S., Tsai, C.-C., Nieuwboer, W., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Baaren, R. B. (2011). When Pinocchio acts like a human, a wooden hand becomes embodied. Action co-representation for non-biological agents. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1373–1377. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.022.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Neely, J. H., & Kahan, T. A. (2001). Is semantic activation automatic? A critical re-evaluation. In H. L. Roediger, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 69–93). Washington, DC: US: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10394-005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Prinz, W. (1990). Relationships between perception and action. In D. O. Neumann & P. D. W. Prinz (Eds.), A common coding approach to perception and action. Berlin: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-75348-0_7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and Action Planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 129–154. doi:10.1080/713752551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Prinz, W. (2015). Task representation in individual and joint settings. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00268.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Scheibe, K. E., Shaver, P. R., & Carrier, S. C. (1967). Color association values and response interference on variants of the Stroop test. Acta Psychologica, 26, 286–295. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(67)90028-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 70–76. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Sebanz, N., & Knoblich, G. (2009). Prediction in joint action: what, when, and where. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(2), 353–367. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01024.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2003). Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own? Cognition, 88(3), B11–B21. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00043-X.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2005). How two share a task: corepresenting stimulus-response mappings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6), 1234–1246. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1234.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Sebanz, N., Voinov, P., & Knoblich, G. (2015). Spatial perspective taking in the context of joint action. Cognitive Processing, 16, S25-S25.Google Scholar
  43. Sellaro, R., Dolk, T., Colzato, L. S., Liepelt, R., & Hommel, B. (2015). Referential coding does not rely on location features: evidence for a nonspatial joint Simon effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 41(1), 186–195. doi:10.1037/a0038548.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Sharma, D., Booth, R., Brown, R., & Huguet, P. (2010). Exploring the temporal dynamics of social facilitation in the Stroop task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 52–58. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.1.52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Stenzel, A., Chinellato, E., Tirado, A., del Pobil, Á. P., Lappe, M., & Liepelt, R. (2012). When humanoid robots become human-like interaction partners: corepresentation of robotic actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1073–1077. doi:10.1037/a0029493.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Stock, A., & Stock, C. (2004). A short history of ideo-motor action. Psychological Research, 68(2–3), 176–188. doi:10.1007/s00426-003-0154-5.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Stroop, R. J. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Tsai, C. C., & Brass, M. (2007). Does the human motor system simulate Pinocchio’s actions? Coacting with a human hand versus a wooden hand in a dyadic interaction. Psychological Science, 18(12), 1058–1062. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02025.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Tsai, C. C., Kuo, W.-J., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (2008). Action co-representation is tuned to other humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(11), 2015–2024. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20144.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. van Schie, H. T., van Waterschoot, B. M., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Understanding action beyond imitation: reversed compatibility effects of action observation in imitation and joint action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(6), 1493–1500. doi:10.1037/a0011750.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2008). Automatic and controlled response inhibition: associative Learning in the go/no-go and stop-signal paradigms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(4), 649–672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Vlainic, E., Liepelt, R., Colzato, L. S., Prinz, W., & Hommel, B. (2010). The virtual co-actor: the social Simon effect does not rely on online feedback from the other. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 208. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00208.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Warren, R. E. (1972). Stimulus encoding and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94(1), 90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Warren, R. E. (1974). Association, directionality, and stimulus encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(1), 151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Welsh, T. N., Higgins, L., Ray, M., & Weeks, D. J. (2007). Seeing vs. believing: is believing sufficient to activate the processes of response co-representation? Human Movement Science, 26(6), 853–866. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.06.003.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Center for Mind/Brain Sciences (CIMeC), University of TrentoRoveretoItaly

Personalised recommendations