What you see and what you are told: an action-specific effect that is unaffected by explicit feedback

  • Zachary R. King
  • Nathan L. Tenhundfeld
  • Jessica K. Witt
Original Article

Abstract

A critical question for theories of spatial vision concerns the nature of the inputs to perception. The action-specific account asserts that information related to action, specifically a perceiver’s ability to perform the intended action, is one of these sources of information. This claim challenges assumptions about the mind in general and perception in particular, and not surprisingly, has been met with much resistance. Alternative explanations include that these effects are due to response bias, rather than genuine differences in perception. Using a paradigm in which ease to block a ball impacts estimated speed of the ball, participants were given explicit feedback about their perceptual judgements to test the response bias alternative. Despite the feedback, the action-specific effect still persisted, thus ruling out a response-bias interpretation. Coupled with other research ruling out additional alternative explanations, the current findings offer an important step towards the claim that a person’s ability to act truly influences spatial perception.

Keywords

Action-specific perception Spatial perception Response bias Feedback 

Supplementary material

426_2017_848_MOESM1_ESM.docx (183 kb)
Supplementary material 1 (DOCX 182 KB)

References

  1. Aberg, K. C., Tartaglia, E. M., & Herzog, M. H. (2009). Perceptual learning with Chevrons requires a minimal number of trials, transfers to untrained directions, but does not require sleep. Vision Research, 49, 2087–2094.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and Social Pressure. Scientific American, 193(5), 31–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Biederman, I., & Shiffrar, M. M. (1987). Sexing day-old chicks: a case study and expert systems analysis of a difficult perceptual-learning task. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 13, 640–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Choe, C. S., Welch, R. B., Gilford, R. M., & Joula, J. F. (1975). The “ventriloquist effect”: visual dominance or response bias? Perception and Psychophysics, 18(1), 55–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Durgin, F. H., Baird, J. A., Greenburg, M., Russell, R., Shaughnessy, K., & Waymouth, S. (2009). Who is being deceived? The experimental demands of wearing a backpack. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(5), 964–969. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.5.964.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: evaluating the evidence for ‘top-down’ effects. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e229. doi:10.1017/S0140525X15000965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Foley, J. M. (1977). Effect of distance information and range on two indices of visually perceived distance. Perception, 6(4), 449–460.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Gogel, W. C. (1990). A theory of phenomenal geometry and its applications. Perception and Psychophysics, 48(2), 105–123.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Gray, R. (2013). Being selective at the plate: processing dependence between perceptual variables relates to hitting goals and performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 39(4), 1124–1142.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Grove, P. M., Ashton, J., Kawachi, Y., & Sakurai, K. (2012). Auditory transients do not affect visual sensitivity in discriminating between objective streaming and bouncing events. Journal of Vision, 12(8), 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Karni, A., Tanne, D., Rubenstein, B. S., Askenasy, J. J., & Sagi, D. (1994). Dependence on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a perceptual skill. Science, 265(5172), 679–682.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Kirsch, W., & Kunde, W. (2014). Impact of planned movement direction on judgments of visual locations. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung, 78, 705–720.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Lee, Y., Lee, S., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2012). An archer’s perceived form scales the “hitableness” of archery targets. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1125–1131.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Object manipulation and motion perception: evidence of an influence of action planning on visual processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1062–1071.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Lippert, M., Logothetis, N. K., & Kayser, C. (2007). Improvement of visual contrast detection by a simultaneous sound. Brain Research, 1173, 102–109.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Loomis, J. M. (2016). Proposed applications of research on action-specific effects are premature. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(1), 77–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Loomis, J. M., & Philbeck, J. W. (2008). Measuring perception with spatial updating and action. In R. L. Klatzky, M. Behrmann & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), Embodiment, ego-space, and action (pp. 1–44). Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  19. Musseler, J., & Hommel, B. (1997). Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 23(3), 861–872.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Odgaard, E. C., Arieh, Y., & Marks, L. E. (2003). Cross-modal enhancement of perceived brightness: sensory interaction versus response bias. Perception and Psychophysics, 65, 123–132.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Philbeck, J. W., & Witt, J. K. (2015). Action-specific influences on perception and post-perceptual processes: present controversies and future directions. Psychological Bulletin, 141(6), 1120–1144. doi:10.1037/a0039738.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  22. Proffitt, D. R. (2008). An action-specific approach to spatial perception. In R. L. Klatzky, B. MacWhinney & M. Behrmann (Eds.), Embodiment, ego-space, and action (pp. 179–202). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  23. Proffitt, D. R., & Linkenauger, S. A. (2013). Perception viewed as a phenotypic expression. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 171–198). Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Rosenthal, O., Shimojo, S., & Shams, L. (2009). Sound-induced flash illusion is resistant to feedback training. Brian Topography, 21, 185–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Taylor, J. E. T., Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2011). When walls are no longer barriers: perception of wall height in parkour. Perception, 40(6), 757–760. doi:10.1068/P6855.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. van der Kamp, J., Withagen, R., & de Witt, M. M. (2013). Cultural and learning differences in the Judd illusion. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 75, 1027–1038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Wesp, R., & Gasper, J. (2012). Is size misperception of targets simply justification for poor performance? Perception, 41(8), 994–996. doi:10.1068/p7281.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Wilson, T. D., Reinhard, D. A., Westgate, E. C., Gilbert, D. T., Ellerbeck, N., Hahn, C., & Shaked, A. (2014). Just think: the challenges of the disengaged mind. Science, 345(July 4), 75–77.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Witt, J. K. (2011). Action’s Effect on Perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 201–206. doi:10.1177/0963721411408770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Witt, J. K. (2016a). Action potential influences spatial perception: evidence for genuine top-down effects on perception. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review.Google Scholar
  31. Witt, J. K. (2016b). Perception and action. In J. T. Wixted (Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience, fourth edition (Fourth ed., Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  32. Witt, J. K. (2016c). Spatial biases from action. In T. Hubbard (Ed.), Spatial biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Witt, J. K., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., & Proffitt, D. R. (2008). Putting to a bigger hole: golf performance relates to perceived size. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15(3), 581–585. doi:10.3758/15.3.581.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2005). See the ball, hit the ball - Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average. Psychological Science, 16(12), 937–938. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01640.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Witt, J. K., Schuck, D. M., & Taylor, J. E. T. (2011). Action-specific effects underwater. Perception, 40(5), 530–537. doi:10.1068/P6910.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2010). Performance and ease influence perceived speed. Perception, 39(10), 1341–1353. doi:10.1068/P6699.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2012). Does ease to block a ball affect perceived ball speed? Examination of alternative hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(5), 1202–1214. doi:10.1037/a0026512.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2013a). Catching ease influences perceived speed: evidence for action-specific effects from action-based measures. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 1364–1370.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2013b). Response bias cannot explain action-specific effects: evidence from compliant and non-compliant participants. Perception, 42, 138–152.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Witt, J. K., Sugovic, M., & Dodd, M. D. (2016). Action-specific perception of speed is independent of attention. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 78(3), 880–890. doi:10.3758/s13414-015-1047-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Witt, J. K., Sugovic, M., & Taylor, J. E. T. (2012). Action-specific effects in a social context: others’ abilities influence perceived speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 715–725. doi:10.1037/a0026261.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Witt, J. K., Sugovic, M., Tenhundfeld, N. T., & King, Z. R. (2016). An action-specific effect on perception that avoids all pitfalls. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e261. doi:10.1017/S0140525X15002563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Witt, J. K., Tenhundfeld, N. T., & Bielak, A. A. M. (2017). Dissociating perception from judgment in the action-specific effect of blocking ease on perceived speed. Attention Perception and Psychophysics, 79(1), 283–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Woods, A. J., Philbeck, J. W., & Danoff, J. V. (2009). The various perceptions of distance: an alternative view of how effort affects distance judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1104–1117. doi:10.1037/a0013622.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2017

Authors and Affiliations

  • Zachary R. King
    • 1
  • Nathan L. Tenhundfeld
    • 1
  • Jessica K. Witt
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyColorado State UniversityFort CollinsUSA

Personalised recommendations