Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 81, Issue 5, pp 1020–1034 | Cite as

Route planning with transportation network maps: an eye-tracking study

  • Elise Grison
  • Valérie Gyselinck
  • Jean-Marie Burkhardt
  • Jan Malte Wiener
Original Article

Abstract

Planning routes using transportation network maps is a common task that has received little attention in the literature. Here, we present a novel eye-tracking paradigm to investigate psychological processes and mechanisms involved in such a route planning. In the experiment, participants were first presented with an origin and destination pair before we presented them with fictitious public transportation maps. Their task was to find the connecting route that required the minimum number of transfers. Based on participants’ gaze behaviour, each trial was split into two phases: (1) the search for origin and destination phase, i.e., the initial phase of the trial until participants gazed at both origin and destination at least once and (2) the route planning and selection phase. Comparisons of other eye-tracking measures between these phases and the time to complete them, which depended on the complexity of the planning task, suggest that these two phases are indeed distinct and supported by different cognitive processes. For example, participants spent more time attending the centre of the map during the initial search phase, before directing their attention to connecting stations, where transitions between lines were possible. Our results provide novel insights into the psychological processes involved in route planning from maps. The findings are discussed in relation to the current theories of route planning.

Keywords

Cognitive Load Task Complexity Travel Salesman Problem Destination Station Pupil Size 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

This study was carried out thanks to a grant for 2 months international exchange from the Paris Descartes University Doctoral School (ED 261, Cognition, Comportements, Conduites Humaines).

Compliance with ethical standards

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for the study was granted by the Bournemouth University committee. All procedures performed in this study were in accordance with 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments as well as comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that there were no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., & Uttal, D. H. (1998). Road climbing: Principles governing asymmetric route choices on maps. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 18(3), 251–264. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1998.0095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bailenson, J. N., Shum, M. S., & Uttal, D. H. (2000). The initial segment strategy: A heuristic for route selection. Memory and Cognition, 28(2), 306–318. doi: 10.3758/BF03213808.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91(2), 276–292.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Beck, M. R., Trenchard, M., van Lamsweerde, A., Goldstein, R. R., & Lohrenz, M. (2012). Searching in clutter: Visual attention strategies of expert pilots. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 56, 1411–1415. doi: 10.1177/1071181312561400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Benshoof, J. A. (1970). Characteristics of drivers’ route selection behavior. Traffic Engineering and Control, 11, 604–606.Google Scholar
  6. Bovy, P. H. L., & Stern, E. (1990). Route choice: Wayfinding in transport networks. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 27, 338–339. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-033-4.Google Scholar
  7. Brunyé, T. T., Andonova, E., Meneghetti, C., Noordzij, M. L., Pazzaglia, F., Wienemann, R., … Taylor, H. A. (2012a). Planning routes around the world: International evidence for southern route preferences. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 32(4), 297–304. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.05.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brunyé, T. T., Gagnon, S. A., Waller, D., Hodgson, E., Tower-Richardi, S., & Taylor, H. A. (2012b). Up north and down south: Implicit associations between topography and cardinal direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 1880–1894. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.663393.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., Gardony, A. L., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). North is up(hill): Route planning heuristics in real-world environments. Memory and Cognition, 38(6), 700–712. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.6.700.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Brunyé, T. T., & Taylor, H. A. (2009). When goals constrain: Eye movements and memory for goal-oriented map study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23(6), 772–787. doi: 10.1002/acp.1508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Casakin, H., Barkowsky, T., Klippel, A., & Freksa, C. (2000). Schematic maps as way finding aids. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition II: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 54–71). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cazzato, V., Basso, D., Cutini, S., & Bisiacchi, P. (2010). Gender differences in visuospatial planning: An eye movements study. Behavioural Brain Research, 206(2), 177–183. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Chowdhury, S., & Ceder, A. (2013). The effect of interchange attributes on public-transport users’ intention to use routes involving transfers. Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 2(1), 5–13. doi: 10.11648/j.pbs.20130201.12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Conroy Dalton, R. (2003). The secret is to follow your nose: Route path selection and angularity. Environment and Behavior, 35(1), 107–131. doi: 10.1177/0013916502238867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. de Condappa, O., & Wiener, J. (2016). Human place and response learning: Navigation strategy selection, pupil size and gaze behavior. Psychological Research, 80(1), 82–93.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Ettema, D., Friman, M., Gärling, T., Olsson, L. E., & Fujii, S. (2012). How in-vehicle activities affect work commuters’ satisfaction with public transport. Journal of Transport Geography, 24, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/j.jtrangeo.2012.02.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Farr, A. C., Kleinschmidt, T., Yarlagadda, P., & Mengersen, K. (2012). Wayfinding: A simple concept, a complex process. Transport Reviews, 32(6), 715–743. doi: 10.1080/01441647.2012.712555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Freksa, C. (1999). Spatial aspects of task-specific wayfinding maps: A representation-theoretic perspective. In J. S. Gero & B. Tversky (Eds.), Visual and spatial reasoning in design (pp. 15–32). Sydney: Key Centre of Design Computing and Cognition, University of Sydney.Google Scholar
  19. Friman, M. (2010). Affective dimensions of the waiting experience. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 13(3), 197–205. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2010.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Garland, H. C., Haynes, J. J., & Grubb, G. C. (1979). Transit map color coding and street detail. Environment and Behavior, 11(2), 162–184. doi: 10.1177/0013916579112002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gärling, T., & Gärling, E. (1988). Distance minimization in downtown pedestrian shopping. Environment and Planning A, 20, 547–554. doi: 10.1068/a200547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gärling, T., Lindberg, E., & Mäntylä, T. (1983). Orientation in buildings: Effects of familiarity, visual access, and orientation aids. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 177–186. doi: 10.1037/00219010.68.1.177.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gärling, T., Säisä, J., Book, A., & Lindberg, E. (1986). The spatiotemporal sequencing of everyday activities in the large-scale environment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 6(4), 261–280. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(86)80001-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Golledge, R. (1995). Path selection and route preference in human navigation: A progress report. In A. U. Frank & W. Kuhn (Eds.), Spatial information theory: A theoretical basis for GIS (COSIT’95). Lecture notes in computer science, no. 988 (pp. 207–222). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Golledge, R. G. (1999). Human cognitive maps and wayfinding. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 1–45). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Guo, Z. (2011). Mind the map! The impact of transit maps on path choice in public transit. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 45(7), 625–639. doi: 10.1016/j.tra.2011.04.001.Google Scholar
  27. Hayes-Roth, B., & Hayes-Roth, F. (1979). A cognitive model of planning. Cognitive Science, 3(4), 275–310. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0304_1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A, Jr., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 210. doi: 10.1037/00961523.25.1.210.Google Scholar
  29. Hine, J., & Scott, J. (2000). Seamless, accessible travel: Users’ views of the public transport journey and interchange. Transport Policy, 7(3), 217–226. doi: 10.1016/S0967-070X(00)00022-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hölscher, C., Tenbrink, T., & Wiener, J. M. (2011). Would you follow your own route description? Cognitive strategies in urban route planning. Cognition, 121(2), 228–247. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.06.005.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329–354. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.87.4.829.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. MacGregor, J. N., Chronicle, E. P., & Ormerod, T. C. (2004). Convex hull or crossing avoidance? Solution heuristics in the traveling salesperson problem. Memory and Cognition, 32(2), 260–270. doi: 10.3758/BF03196857.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. MacGregor, J. N., & Chu, Y. (2011). Human performance on the traveling salesman and related problems: A review. The Journal of Problem Solving, 3(2), 1–29. doi: 10.7771/1932-6246.1090.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. MacGregor, J. N., & Ormerod, T. (1996). Human performance on the traveling salesman problem. Perception and Psychophysics, 58(4), 527–539. doi: 10.3758/BF03213088.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. MacGregor, J. N., Ormerod, T. C., & Chronicle, E. P. (1999). Spatial and contextual factors in human performance on the travelling salesperson problem. Perception, 28(11), 1417–1427. doi: 10.1068/p2863.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Meilinger, T., Hölscher, C., Büchner, S. J., & Brösamle, M. (2007). How much information do you need? Schematic maps in wayfinding and self localisation. In T. Barkowsky (Ed.), Spatial cognition V—reasoning, action, interaction (pp. 381–400). Berlin: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-3-540-75666-8_22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Neider, M. B., & Zelinsky, G. J. (2006). Scene context guides eye movements during visual search. Vision Research, 46(5), 614–621. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.025.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Pomplun, M., Reingold, E. M., & Shen, J. (2001). Investigating the visual span in comparative search: The effects of task difficulty and divided attention. Cognition, 81(2), B57–B67. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00123-8.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Raveau, S., Guo, Z., Muñoz, J. C., & Wilson, N. H. M. (2014). A behavioural comparison of route choice on metro networks: Time, transfers, crowding, topology and socio-demographics. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice, 66, 185–195. doi: 10.1016/j.tra.2014.05.010.Google Scholar
  40. Roberts, M. J. (2008). Underground maps after Beck (2nd ed.). Harrow Weald: Capital Transport Publishing.Google Scholar
  41. Roberts, M. J., Newton, E. J., Lagattolla, F. D., Hughes, S., & Hasler, M. C. (2013). Objective versus subjective measures of Paris Metro map usability: Investigating traditional octolinear versus all-curves schematics. International Journal of Human Computer Studies, 71, 363–386. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhcs.2012.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Sobel, K. V., Gerrie, M. P., Poole, B. J., & Kane, M. J. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and visual search: The roles of top-down and bottom-up processing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14(5), 840–845. doi: 10.3758/BF03194109.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Van Orden, K. F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., & Jung, T. P. (2001). Eye activity correlates of workload during a visuospatial memory task. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 43(1), 111–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Vickers, D., Lee, M. D., Dry, M., & Hughes, P. (2003). The roles of the convex hull and the number of potential intersections in performance on visually presented traveling salesperson problems. Memory and Cognition, 31(7), 1094–1104. doi: 10.3758/BF03196130.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Wiener, J. M., Ehbauer, N. N., & Mallot, H. A. (2009). Planning paths to multiple targets: Memory involvement and planning heuristics in spatial problem solving. Psychological Research, 73(5), 644–658. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0181-3.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Wiener, J. M., & Tenbrink, T. (2008). Traveling salesman problem: The human case. Künstliche Intelligenz. KI und Kognition, 1(08), 18–22.Google Scholar
  47. Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0 a revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1(2), 202–238. doi: 10.3758/BF03200774.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Memory and Cognition Lab, UMR S894Paris Descartes University and INSERMParisFrance
  2. 2.Psychology of Behaviour and Mobility LabIFSTTARVersaillesFrance
  3. 3.Psychology DepartmentBournemouth UniversityPooleUK
  4. 4.Dementia InstituteBournemouth UniversityPooleUK

Personalised recommendations