Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Pushing the rules: effects and aftereffects of deliberate rule violations

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Most of our daily life is organized around rules and social norms. But what makes rules so special? And what if one were to break a rule intentionally? Can we simply free us from the present set of rules or do we automatically adhere to them? How do rule violations influence subsequent behavior? To investigate the effects and aftereffects of violating simple S-R rule, we conducted three experiments that investigated continuous finger-tracking responses on an iPad. Our experiments show that rule violations are distinct from rule-based actions in both response times and movement trajectories, they take longer to initiate and execute, and their movement trajectory is heavily contorted. Data not only show differences between the two types of response (rule-based vs. violation), but also yielded a characteristic pattern of aftereffects in case of rule violations: rule violations do not trigger adaptation effects that render further rule violations less difficult, but every rule violation poses repeated effort on the agent. The study represents a first step towards understanding the signature and underlying mechanisms of deliberate rule violations, they cannot be acted out by themselves, but require the activation of the original rule first. Consequently, they are best understood as reformulations of existing rules that are not accessible on their own, but need to be constantly derived from the original rule, with an add-on that might entail an active tendency to steer away from mental representations that reflect (socially) unwanted behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Shifting intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Conscious and nonconscious information processing: attention and performance XV (pp. 421–452). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arrington, C. M., & Logan, G. D. (2004). The cost of a voluntary task switch. Psychological Science, 15(9), 610–615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arrington, C. M., Weaver, S. M., & Pauker, R. L. (2010). Stimulus-based priming of task choice during voluntary task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(4), 1060–1067.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70, 1–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chermayeff, M., Dupre, J., & Matthew Akers, M. (2012). Marina Abramovic: the artist is present [motion picture]. USA: Show Of Force.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1972). On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology, 3(3), 472–517.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Chase, W. G. (1974). Perceptual coding strategies in the formation and verification of descriptions. Memory and Cognition, 2(1), 101–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fillenbaum, S. (1966). Memory for gist: some relevant variables. Language and Speech, 9(4), 217–227.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T. (1991). How mental systems believe. American Psychologist, 46(2), 107–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratton, G., Coles, M. G., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121(4), 480–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, U., Simmons, J. P., & Todorov, A. (2005). Believe it or not. On the possibility of suspending belief. Psychological Science, 16(7), 566–571.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding: a framework for perception and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, Y., Shencar, Y., & Meiran, N. (2009). Choosing to switch: spontaneous task switching despite associated behavioral costs. Acta Psychologica, 131(2), 120–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, D., & Hommel, B. (2015). An event-based account of conformity. Psychological Science, 26(4), 484–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liefooghe, B., Demanet, J., & Vandierendonck, A. (2010). Persisting activation in voluntary task switching: it all depends on the instructions. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17(3), 381–386.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a stroop-like task. Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 166–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayo, R., Schul, Y., & Burnstein, E. (2004). “I am not guilty” vs. “I am innocent”: successful negation may depend on the schema used for its encoding. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(4), 433–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Notebaert, W., Houtman, F., Opstal, F. V., Gevers, W., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2009). Post-error slowing: an orienting account. Cognition, 111(2), 275–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfister, R. (2013). Breaking the rules: cognitive conflict during deliberate rule violations. Berlin: Logos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9(2), 74–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., Wirth, R., Dignath, D., & Kunde, W. (2014). Thinking with portals: revisiting kinematic cues to intention. Cognition, 133(2), 464–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pfister, R., Wirth, R., Schwarz, K., Steinhauser, M., & Kunde, W. (submitted). Burdens of non-conformity: motor execution reveals cognitive conflict during deliberate rule violations. Cognition.

  • Reason, J. (1990). Human error. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reason, J. (1995). Understanding adverse events: human factors. Quality in Health Care, 4(2), 80–89.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schroder, H. S., Moran, T. P., Moser, J. S., & Altmann, E. M. (2012). When the rules are reversed: action-monitoring consequences of reversing stimulus–response mappings. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 12(4), 629–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 220–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandierendonck, A., Demanet, J., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2012). A chain-retrieval model for voluntary task switching. Cognitive Psychology, 65(2), 241–283.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wason, P. C. (1959). The processing of positive and negative information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 11(2), 92–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M. (2009). How to think, say, or do precisely the worst thing for any occasion. Science, 325(5936), 48–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wegner, D. M., Coulton, G. F., & Wenzlaff, R. (1985). The transparency of denial: briefing in the debriefing paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(2), 338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, R., et al. (2015). Through the portal: Effect anticipation in the central bottleneck. Acta Psychologica. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.07.007.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wirth, R., Pfister, R., & Kunde, W. (2015). Asymmetric transfer effects between cognitive and affective task disturbances. Cognition and Emotion, 1–18. doi:10.1080/02699931.2015.1009002.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Wirth.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wirth, R., Pfister, R., Foerster, A. et al. Pushing the rules: effects and aftereffects of deliberate rule violations. Psychological Research 80, 838–852 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0690-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-015-0690-9

Keywords

Navigation