Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 80, Issue 5, pp 860–876 | Cite as

Proactive control of irrelevant task rules during cued task switching

  • Julie M. Bugg
  • Todd S. Braver
Original Article

Abstract

In task-switching paradigms, participants are often slower on incongruent than congruent trials, a pattern known as the task-rule congruency effect. This effect suggests that irrelevant task rules or associated responses may be retrieved automatically in spite of task cues. The purpose of the present study was to examine whether the task-rule congruency effect may be modulated via manipulations intended to induce variation in proactive control. Manipulating the proportion of congruent to incongruent trials strongly influenced the magnitude of the task-rule congruency effect. The effect was significantly reduced in a mostly incongruent list relative to a mostly congruent list, a pattern that was observed for not only biased but also 50 % congruent items. This finding implicates a role for global attentional control processes in the task-rule congruency effect. In contrast, enhancing the preparation of relevant (cued) task rules by the provision of a monetary incentive substantially reduced mixing costs but did not affect the task-rule congruency effect. These patterns support the view that there may be multiple routes by which proactive control can influence task-switching performance; however, only select routes appear to influence the automatic retrieval of irrelevant task rules.

Keywords

Incongruent Trial Irrelevant Dimension Switch Trial Task Rule Proactive Control 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (R37 MH066078). The authors are grateful to Bridgette Shamleffer, Marie Krug, Kevin Oksanen, and Jason Li for assistance with data collection and programming.

References

  1. Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., & Hsieh, S. (1994). Switching intentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance, XV (pp. 421–452). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Askren, M. K. A. (2010). You can’t have it both ways: An examination of congruency effects in task switching. Unpublished Dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.Google Scholar
  3. Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 379–384.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Blais, C., & Bunge, S. (2010). Behavioral and neural evidence for item-specific performance monitoring. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2758–2767.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Blais, C., Harris, M. B., Guerrero, J. V., & Bunge, S. A. (2012). Rethinking the role of automaticity in cognitive control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 268–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item-specific adaptation and the conflict monitoring hypothesis: a computational model. Psychological Review, 114, 1076–1086.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Braver, T. S., Gray, J. R., & Burgess, G. C. (2007). Explaining the many varieties of working memory variation: dual mechanisms of cognitive control. In A. R. A. Conway, C. Jarrold, M. J. Kane, A. Miyake, & J. N. Towse (Eds.), Variation in working memory (pp. 76–106). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Braverman, A., & Meiran, N. (2015). Conflict control in task conflict and response conflict. Psychological Research, 79, 238–248.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bugg, J. M. (2012). Dissociating levels of cognitive control: the case of Stroop interference. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 302–309. doi: 10.1177/0963721412453586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bugg, J. M. (2014). Conflict-triggered top-down control: default mode, last resort, or no such thing? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 567–587. doi: 10.1037/a0035032.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bugg, J. M., & Chanani, S. (2011). List-wide control is not entirely elusive: evidence from picture-word Stroop. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 930–936. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0112-y.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Bugg, J. M., & Crump, M. J. C. (2012). In support of a distinction between voluntary and stimulus-driven control: a review of the literature on proportion congruent effects. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition, 3, 1–16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bugg, J. M., Diede, N. T., Cohen-Shikora, E. R., & Szelmecy, D. (2015). Expectations and experience: dissociable bases for cognitive control? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000106.
  15. Bugg, J. M., & Hutchison, K. A. (2013). Converging evidence for control of color-word Stroop interference at the item level. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 433–449. doi: 10.1037/a0029145.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Chanani, S. (2011a). Why it is too early to lose control in accounts of item-specific proportion congruency effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 844–859. doi: 10.1037/a0019957.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Bugg, J. M., Jacoby, L. L., & Toth, J. (2008). Multiple levels of control in the Stroop task. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1484–1494. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.8.1484.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Bugg, J. M., McDaniel, M. A., Scullin, M. K., & Braver, T. S. (2011b). Revealing list-level control in the Stroop task by uncovering its benefits and a cost. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 1595–1606. doi: 10.1037/a0024670.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Chiew, K. S., & Braver, T. S. (2013). Temporal dynamics of motivation-cognitive control interactions revealed by high-resolution pupillometry. Frontiers in Psychology,. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00015.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. De Jong, R. (2000). An intention–activation account of residual switch costs. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 357–376). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  21. De Pisapia, N., & Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69, 1322–1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Dishon-Berkovits, M., & Algom, D. (2000). The Stroop effect: It is not the robust phenomenon that you have thought it to be. Memory and Cognition, 28, 1437–1449.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Egner, T. (2007). Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 380–390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fagot, C. (1994). Chronometric investigations of task switching. Unpublished Dissertation, University of California-San Diego, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
  25. Frings, C., & Rothermund, K. (2011). To be or not to be…included in an event file: integration and retrieval of distractors in stimulus-response episodes is influenced by perceptual grouping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1209–1227.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Goschke, T. (2000). Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary persistence in task-set switching. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and performance XVIII: control of cognitive processes (pp. 331–355). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  27. Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1992). Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation and responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 480–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Heitz, R. P. (2014). The speed-accuracy tradeoff: history, physiology, methodology, and behavior. Frontiers in Neuroscience,. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00150.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Hommel, B. (1994). Spontaneous decay of response-code activation. Psychological Research, 56, 261–268.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Hutchison, K. A. (2011). The interactive effects of listwide control, item-based control, and working memory capacity on Stroop performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 851–860.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Jersild, A. T. (1927). Mental set and switch. Archives of Psychology, Whole No. 89.Google Scholar
  32. Jimura, K., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Age-related shifts in brain activity dynamics during task switching. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 1420–1431. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp206.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Jimura, K., Locke, H. S., & Braver, T. S. (2010). Prefontal cortex mediation of cognitive enhancement in rewarding motivational contexts. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 8871–8876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kessler, Y., & Meiran, N. (2010). The reaction time task-rule congruency effect is not affected by working memory load: further support for the activated long-term memory hypothesis. Psychological Research, 74, 388–399.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching: a review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 849–874.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Kiesel, A., Wendt, M., & Peters, A. (2007). Task switching: on the origins of response congruency effects. Psychological Research, 71, 117–125.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: a review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 17, 1–4.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: the relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 219–234.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Logan, G. D. (1980). Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: theory and data. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 523–553.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Logan, G. D., & Zbrodoff, N. J. (1979). When it helps to be misled: facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop –like task. Memory and Cognition, 7, 166–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Logan, G. D., Zbrodoff, N. J., & Williamson, J. (1984). Strategies in the color-word Stroop task. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 135–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Lowe, D., & Mitterer, J. O. (1982). Selective and divided attention in a Stroop task. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 684–700.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1124–1140.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Meiran, N. (1996). Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1423–1442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Meiran, N. (2000). Modeling cognitive control in task-switching. Psychological Research, 63, 234–249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Meiran, N. (2005). Task rule congruency and Simon-like effects in switching between spatial tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 58A, 1023–1041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Meiran, N., Hsieh, S., & Dimov, E. (2010). Resolving task rule incongruence during task switching by competitor rule suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 992–1002.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Meiran, N., & Kessler, Y. (2008). The task rule congruency effect in task switching reflects activated long term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 137–157.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Meiran, N., Kessler, Y., & Adi-Japha, E. (2008). Control by action representation and input selection (CARIS): a theoretical framework for task switching. Psychological Research, 72, 473–500.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Melara, R. D., & Algom, D. (2003). Driven by information: a tectonic theory of Stroop effects. Psychological Review, 110, 422–471.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Monsell, S., & Mizon, G. A. (2006). Can the task cueing paradigm measure an “endogenous” task set reconfiguration process? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 493–516.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Monsell, S., Sumner, P., & Waters, H. (2003). Task-set reconfiguration with predictable and unpredictable task switches. Memory and Cognition, 31, 327–342.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Nieuwenhuis, S., & Monsell, S. (2002). Residual costs in task switching: testing the failure-to- engage hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 86–92.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Olejnik, S., & Algina, J. (2003). Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs. Psychological Methods, 8, 434–447.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Rogers, R. D., & Monsell, S. (1995). The cost of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Rubin, O., & Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 31, 1477–1491.Google Scholar
  58. Ruge, H., Jamadar, S., Zimmerman, U., & Karayanidis, F. (2013). The many faces of preparatory control in task switching: reviewing a decade of fMRI research. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 12–35.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Schmidt, J. R. (2013). Temporal learning and list-level proportion congruency: conflict adaptation or learning when to respond? PLoS One,. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.008232012.Google Scholar
  60. Schmidt, J. R. (2014). List-level transfer effects in temporal learning: further complications for the list-level proportion congruent effect. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 373–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Schmidt, J. R., & Besner, D. (2008). The Stroop effect: Why proportion congruence has nothing to do with congruency and everything to do with contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 514–523.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Schneider, D. W. (2014). Isolating a mediated route for response congruency effects in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000049.
  63. Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2005). Modeling task switching without switching tasks: a short-term priming account of explicitly cued performance. Psychology: General, 134, 343––367.Google Scholar
  64. Schneider, D. W., & Logan, G. D. (2010). The target of task switching. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 129–133.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Schuch, S., & Koch, I. (2003). The role of response selection for inhibition of task sets in task shifting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 92–105.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Sudevan, P., & Taylor, D. A. (1987). The cueing and priming of cognitive operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 89–103.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Toth, J. P., Levine, B., Stuss, D. T., Oh, A., Winocur, G., & Meiran, N. (1995). Dissociation of processes underlying spatial SR compatibility: evidence for the independent influence of what and where. Consciousness and Cognition, 4(4), 483–501.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Tzelgov, J. (1997). Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: a theoretical note. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 441–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., & Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 601–626.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Verguts, T., & Notebaert, W. (2008). Hebbian learning of cognitive control: dealing with specific and nonspecific adaptation. Psychological Review, 115, 518–525.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Waszak, F., Hommel, B., & Allport, A. (2003). Task-switching and long term priming: role of episodic stimulus-task bindings in task-shift costs. Cognitive Psychology, 46, 361–413.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Waszak, F., Pfister, R., & Kiesel, A. (2013). Top-down versus bottom-up: when instructions overcome automatic retrieval. Psychological Research, 77, 611–617.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Wendt, M., & Kiesel, A. (2008). The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks. Psychological Research, 72, 425–432.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Wendt, M., & Luna-Rodriguez, A. (2009). Conflict-frequency affects flanker-interference. Experimental Psychology, 56, 206–217.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. West, R., & Baylis, G. C. (1998). Effect of increased response dominance and contextual disintegration on the Stroop interference effect in older adults. Psychology and Aging, 13, 206–217.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Yamaguchi, M., & Proctor, R. W. (2011). Automaticity without extensive training: the role of memory retrieval in implementation of task-defined rules. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 347–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, Campus Box 1125Washington University in St. LouisSt. LouisUSA

Personalised recommendations