Psychological Research

, Volume 80, Issue 1, pp 113–127 | Cite as

Mental rotation training: transfer and maintenance effects on spatial abilities

  • Chiara Meneghetti
  • Erika Borella
  • Francesca Pazzaglia
Original Article

Abstract

One of the aims of research in spatial cognition is to examine whether spatial skills can be enhanced. The goal of the present study was thus to assess the benefit and maintenance effects of mental rotation training in young adults. Forty-eight females took part in the study: 16 were randomly assigned to receive the mental rotation training (based on comparing pairs of 2D or 3D objects and rotation games), 16 served as active controls (performing parallel non-spatial activities), and 16 as passive controls. Transfer effects to both untrained spatial tasks (testing both object rotation and perspective taking) and visual and verbal tasks were examined. Across the training sessions, the group given mental rotation training revealed benefits in the time it took to make judgments when comparing 3D and 2D objects, but their mental rotation speed did not improve. When compared with the other groups, the mental rotation training group did show transfer effects, however, in tasks other than those practiced (i.e., in object rotation and perspective-taking tasks), and these benefits persisted after 1 month. The training had no effect on visual or verbal tasks. These findings are discussed from the spatial cognition standpoint and with reference to the (rotation) training literature.

Notes

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Francesco Ferrari and Antonio Di Maggio for helping to collect the data.

References

  1. Allen, G. L., Kirasic, K. C., Dobson, S. H., Long, R. G., & Beck, S. (1996). Predicting environmental learning from spatial abilities: an indirect route. Intelligence, 22, 327–355. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90026-4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Borella, E., Carretti, B., Cantarella, A., Riboldi, F., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni, R. (2013). Benefits of training visuospatial working memory in young-old and old-old. Developmental Psychology,. doi: 10.1037/a0033818.Google Scholar
  3. Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., Hutchison, S., Perrig-Chiello, P., Däpp, C., Müller, M., et al. (2008). Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychology and Aging, 23, 743–753. doi: 10.1037/a0014342.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. De Lisi, R., & Cammarano, D. M. (1996). Computer experience and gender differences in undergraduate mental rotation performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 12, 351–361. doi: 10.1016/0747-5632(96)00013-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princenton: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
  6. Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2006). Separating “rotators” from “nonrotators” in the mental rotation test: a multigroup latent class analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 261–293. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr4103_2.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Guilford, J. P., & Zimmerman, W. S. (1947). The Guilford-Zimmerman aptitude survey. Orange: Sheridan Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  8. Hegarty, M., Montello, D. R., Richardson, A. E., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34, 151–176. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Hegarty, M., & Waller, D. (2004). A dissociation between mental rotation and perspective-taking spatial abilities. Intelligence, 32, 175–191. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2003.12.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Hegarty, M., & Waller, D. (2006). Individual differences in spatial abilities. In P. Shah & A. Miyake (Eds.), Handbook of visuospatial thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  11. Heil, M., Rösler, F., Link, M., & Bajric, J. (1998). What is improved if a mental rotation task is repeated: the efficiency of memory access, or the speed of a transformation routine? Psychological Research, 61, 99–106. doi: 10.1007/s004260050016.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Jansen, P., Wiedenbauer, G., & Hahn, N. (2010). Manual rotation training improves direction estimations in a virtual environmental space. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 6–17. doi: 10.1080/09541440802678487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Kozhevnikov, M., & Hegarty, M. (2001). A dissociation between object manipulation spatial ability and spatial orientation ability. Memory & Cognition, 29, 745–756. doi: 10.3758/BF03200477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Laski, E. V., Casey, B., Yu, Q., Dulaney, A., Heyman, M., & Dearing, E. (2013). Spatial skills as a predictor of first-grade girls’ use of higher level arithmetic strategies. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Leone, G., Taine, M. C., & Droulez, J. (1993). The influence of long-term practice on mental rotation of 3-D objects. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 241–255. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(93)90008-S.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: a meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498. doi: 10.2307/1130467.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Lohman, D. F. (1988). Spatial abilities as traits, processes and knowledge. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 181–248). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Moreau, D. (2012). Differentiating two- from three-dimensional mental rotation training effects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. iFirst,. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.744761.Google Scholar
  19. Moreau, D., Clerc, J., Mansy-Dannay, A., & Guerrien, A. (2012). Enhancing spatial ability through sport practice: Evidence for an effect of motor training on mental rotation performance. Journal of Individual Differences, 33, 83–88. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Pazzaglia, F., & Meneghetti, C. (2012). Spatial text processing in relation to spatial abilities and spatial styles. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 24, 972–980. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2012.725716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Sartori, G., Job, R., & Tressoldi, P. E. (1995). Batteria per la valutazione della dislessia e della disortografia evolutiva [Developmental disortographic and dislexia assessment battery]. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
  22. Shepard, R. N., & Feng, C. A. (1972). A chronometric study of mental paper folding. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 228–243. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(72)90005-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Sims, V. K., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). Domain specificity of spatial expertise: the case of video game players. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16, 97–115. doi: 10.1002/acp.759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Stransky, D., Wilcox, L. M., & Dubrowski, A. (2010). Mental rotation: cross-task training and generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4, 349–360. doi: 10.1037/a0021702.Google Scholar
  25. Terlecki, M. S., Newcombe, N. S., & Little, M. (2008). Durable and generalized effects of spatial experience on mental rotation: gender differences in growth patterns. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 996–1013. doi: 10.1002/acp.1420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Thurstone, L. L., & Jeffrey, T. E. (1956). Flags: A test of space thinking. Chicago: Industrial Relations Centre.Google Scholar
  27. Thurstone, T. G., & Thurstone, L. L. (1963). Primary mental ability. Chicago: Science Research Associates.Google Scholar
  28. Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Tipton, E., Hand, L. L., Alden, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013a). The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 352–402. doi: 10.1037/a0028446.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Uttal, D. H., Miller, D. I., & Newcombe, N. S. (2013b). Exploring and enhancing spatial thinking: links to achievement in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 367–373. doi: 10.1177/0963721413484756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of 3-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Wai, J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2009). Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 817–835. doi: 10.1037/a0016127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Wiedenbauer, G., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Manual training of mental rotation in children. Learning and Instruction, 18, 30–41. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Wright, R., Thompson, W. L., Ganis, G., Newcombe, N. S., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2008). Training generalized spatial skills. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 763–771. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.4.763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  • Chiara Meneghetti
    • 1
  • Erika Borella
    • 1
  • Francesca Pazzaglia
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of General PsychologyUniversity of PadovaPaduaItaly

Personalised recommendations