Psychological Research

, Volume 79, Issue 1, pp 42–63 | Cite as

Is it really search or just matching? The influence of Goodness, number of stimuli and presentation sequence in same–different tasks

  • Frouke Hermens
  • Thomas Lachmann
  • Cees van Leeuwen
Original Article

Abstract

The Goodness of Garner dot patterns has been shown to influence same–different response times in a specific way, which has led to the formulation of a memory search model of pattern comparison. In this model, the space of possible variations of each pattern is searched separately for each pattern in the comparison, resulting in faster response times for patterns that have fewer alternatives. Compared to an alternative explanation based on stimulus encoding plus mental rotation, however, the existing data strongly favor this explanation. To obtain a more constraining set of data to distinguish between the two possible accounts, we extended the original paradigm to a situation in which participants needed to compare three, rather than two patterns and varied the way the stimuli were presented (simultaneously or sequentially). Our findings suggest that neither the memory search nor the encoding plus mental rotation model provides a complete description of the data, and that the effects of Goodness must be understood in a combination of both mechanisms, or in terms of cascades processing.

References

  1. Bell, H. H., & Handel, S. (1976). The role of pattern goodness in the reproduction of backward masked patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2(1):139–150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Berti, S., & Roeber, U. (2013, in press). Encoding into visual working memory: event-related brain potentials reflect automatic processing of seemingly redundant information. Neuroscience Journal.Google Scholar
  3. Berti, S., Geissler, H., Lachmann, T., & Mecklinger, A. (2000). Event-related brain potentials dissociate visual working memory processes under categorial and identical comparison conditions. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research 9(2):147–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carrier, L. M., & Pashler, H. (1995). Attentional limits in memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 21(5):1339–1348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Checkosky, S. F., & Whitlock, D, (1973). Effects of pattern goodness on recognition time in a memory search task. Journal of Experimental Psychology 100(2):341–348.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Clement, D. E., & Varnadoe, K. W. (1967). Pattern uncertainty and the discrimination of visual patterns. Perception and Psychophysics 2(9):427–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Garner, W. R. (1970). Good patterns have few alternatives. American Scientist 58(1):34–42.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Garner, W. R. (1974). The processing of information and structure. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  9. Garner, W. R., & Clement, D. E. (1963). Goodness of pattern and pattern uncertainty. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 2(5):446–452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Garner, W. R., & Sutliff, D. (1974). The effect of goodness onencoding time in visual pattern discrimination. Perception and Psychophysics 16(3):426–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Lachmann, T., & Geissler, H. G. (2002). Memory search instead of template matching? Representation-guided inference in same–different performance. Acta Psychologica 111(3):283–307.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2005a). Individual pattern representations are context independent, but their collective representation is context dependent. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 58(7):1265–1294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2005b). Task-invariant aspects of goodness in perceptual representation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A 58(7):1295–1310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2007). Goodness takes effort: perceptual organization in dual-task settings. Psychological Research 71(2):152–169.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2008). Goodness is central: task-invariance of perceptual organization in a dual-task setting. Japanese Psychological Research 50(4):193–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lachmann, T., & van Leeuwen, C. (2010). Representational economy, not processing speed, determines preferred processing strategy of visual patterns. Acta Psychologica 134(3):290–298.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods 44(2):314–324.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Pomerantz, J. R. (1977). Pattern goodness and speed of encoding. Memory and Cognition 5(2):235–241.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Rauschenberger, R., & Yantis, S. (2006). Perceptual encoding efficiency in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 135(1):116–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ruthruff, E., Miller, J., & Lachmann, T. (1995). Does mental rotation require central mechanisms?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21(3):552–570.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Sebrechts, M. M., & Garner, W. R. (1981). Stimulus-specific processing consequences of pattern goodness. Memory and Cognition 9(1):41–49.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of donders’ method. Acta Psychologica 30:276–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Takahashi, J., Hidaka, S., Teramoto, W., & Gyoba, J. (2013). Temporal characteristics of the effects of visual pattern redundancy on encoding and storage processes: evidence from rapid serial visual presentation. Psychological Research Google Scholar
  24. Van Zandt, T., & Townsend, J. T. (1993). Self-terminating versus exhaustive processes in rapid visual and memory search: an evaluative review. Perception and Psychophysics 53(5):563–580.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Frouke Hermens
    • 1
  • Thomas Lachmann
    • 2
  • Cees van Leeuwen
    • 3
  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity of AberdeenAberdeenUK
  2. 2.Cognitive and Developmental Psychology Unit, Center for Cognitive ScienceUniversity of KaiserslauternKaiserslauternGermany
  3. 3.Laboratory for Perceptual DynamicsUniversity of LeuvenLeuvenBelgium

Personalised recommendations