Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 78, Issue 5, pp 661–669 | Cite as

Media multitasking and failures of attention in everyday life

  • Brandon C. W. RalphEmail author
  • David R. Thomson
  • James Allan Cheyne
  • Daniel Smilek
Original Article

Abstract

Using a series of online self-report measures, we examine media multitasking, a particularly pervasive form of multitasking, and its relations to three aspects of everyday attention: (1) failures of attention and cognitive errors (2) mind wandering, and (3) attentional control with an emphasis on attentional switching and distractibility. We observed a positive correlation between levels of media multitasking and self-reports of attentional failures, as well as with reports of both spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering. No correlation was observed between media multitasking and self-reported memory failures, lending credence to the hypothesis that media multitasking may be specifically related to problems of inattention, rather than cognitive errors in general. Furthermore, media multitasking was not related with self-reports of difficulties in attention switching or distractibility. We offer a plausible causal structural model assessing both direct and indirect effects among media multitasking, attentional failures, mind wandering, and cognitive errors, with the heuristic goal of constraining and motivating theories of the effects of media multitasking on inattention.

Keywords

Attentional Control Mindfulness Training Attentional Switching Cognitive Error Memory Failure 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) discovery grant to DS, and an Ontario Graduate Scholarship to BR. We would like to thank Jonathan Carriere for programming assistance.

References

  1. Alzahabi, R., & Becker, M. W. (2013). The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. doi: 10.1037/a0031208.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cain, M. S., & Mitroff, S. R. (2011). Distractor filtering in media multitaskers. Perception, 40(10), 1183–1192. doi: 10.1068/p7017.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: the affective consequences of mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 835–847. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Carriere, J. S., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. doi: 10.1037/a0031438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cheyne, J. A., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2006). Absent-mindedness: lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 578–592. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2005.11.009.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (2002). Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 225–236. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Lin, L. (2009). Breadth-biased versus focused cognitive control in media multitasking behaviours. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 106(37), 15521–15522. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908642106.Google Scholar
  9. MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., Bridwell, D. A., Zanesco, A. P., Jacobs, T. L., et al. (2010). Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science, 21(6), 829–839. doi: 10.1177/0956797610371339.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 188–207. doi: 10.1037/a0018298.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Minear, M., Brasher, F., McCurdy, M., Lewis, J., & Younggren, A. (2013). Working memory, fluid intelligence, and impulsiveness in heavy media multitaskers. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0456-6.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering. Psychological Science, 24(5), 776–781. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459659.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion, 12(3), 442–448. doi: 10.1037/a0026678.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 106(37), 15583–15587. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903620106.Google Scholar
  15. Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford, H., et al. (2012). Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social well-being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 327–336. doi: 10.1037/a0027030.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generations M [superscript 2]: media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds. Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation. Google Scholar
  17. Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). Oops: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), 747–758.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Strayer, D. L., Mederois-Ward, N., & Watson, J. M. (2013). Who multi-tasks and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-tasking ability, impulsivity, and sensation seeking. PLoS ONE, 8(1). doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0054402.
  19. Seli, P., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering minds and wavering rhythms: linking mind wandering and behavioral variability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(1), 1–5. doi: 10.1037/a0030954.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Shih, S.-I. (2013). A null relationship between media multitasking and well-being. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e64508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064508.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Smallwood, J., Beach, E., Schooler, J. W., & Handy, T. C. (2008). Going AWOL in the brain: mind wandering reduces cortical analysis of external events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(3), 458–469. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20037.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Wang, Z., & Tchernev, J. M. (2012). The “myth” of media multitasking: reciprocal dynamics of media multitasking, personal needs, and gratifications. Journal of Communication, 62(3), 493–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01641.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  • Brandon C. W. Ralph
    • 1
    Email author
  • David R. Thomson
    • 1
  • James Allan Cheyne
    • 1
  • Daniel Smilek
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of WaterlooWaterlooCanada

Personalised recommendations