Psychological Research PRPF

, Volume 74, Issue 4, pp 400–406 | Cite as

Experts see it all: configural effects in action observation

  • Beatriz Calvo-Merino
  • Shantel Ehrenberg
  • Delia Leung
  • Patrick Haggard
Original Article

Abstract

Biological motion perception is influenced by observers’ familiarity with the observed action. Here, we used classical dance as a means to investigate how visual and motor experience modulates perceptual mechanism for configural processing of actions. Although some ballet moves are performed by only one gender, male and female dancers train together and acquire visual knowledge of all ballet moves. Twenty-four expert ballet dancers (12 female) and matched non-expert participants viewed pairs of upright and inverted point light female and common dance movements. Visual discrimination between different exemplars of the same movement presented upright was significantly better in experts than controls, whilst no differences were found when the same stimuli were presented upside down. These results suggest expertise influences configural action processing. Within the expert group, effects were stronger for female participants than for males, whilst no differences were found between movement types. This observer gender effect could suggest an additional role for motor familiarity in action perception, over and above the visual experience. Our results are consistent with a specific motor contribution to configural processing of action.

Notes

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Andrea Serino, Corinne Jola and Manos Tsakiris for initial discussion, to Emma Redding and Laban (London) for help in preparing the stimuli and to the dancers that participated in this study. BC-M and PH were supported by grants from the Leverhulme Trust. BC-M was further supported by the Economical and Social Research Council (ESRC—PTA-026-27-1587) and a City University Fellowship (City University London).

References

  1. Blake, R., & Shiffrar, M. (2007). Perception of human motion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 47–74.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Buccino, G., Lui, F., Canessa, N., Patteri, I., Lagravinese, G., & Benuzzi, F., et al. (2004). Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by non-conspecifics: an fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 114–126.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bukach, C.M, Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M.J. (2006). Beyond faces and modularity: the power of an expertise framework, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(4), 159–166.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16, 1905–1910.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Casile, A., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Nonvisual motor training influences biological motion perception. Current Biology, 16, 69–74.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D. C., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage, 31, 1257–1267.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Cutting, J. E., & Kozlowski, L. T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk—gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353–356.Google Scholar
  9. Cutting, J. E., Moore, C., & Morrison, R. (1988). Masking the motions of human gait. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 339–347.Google Scholar
  10. Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. G., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Farah, M. J., Rabinowitz, C., Quinn, G. E., & Liu, G. T. (2000) Early commitment of neural substrates for face recognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(1–3), 117–123(7).Google Scholar
  12. Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Gore, J. C., & Anderson, A. W. (2000). Expertise for cars and birds recruits brain areas involved in face recognition. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 191–197.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor Learning enhances perceptual judgement: a case for action perception transfer. Psychological Research, 65, 3–14.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Heyes, C. (2003). Four routes of cognitive evolution. Psychological Review, 4, 713–727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hiris, E. (2007). Detection of biological and nonbiological motion. Journal of Vision, 7 (12):4, 1–16.Google Scholar
  16. Hiris, E., Krebeck, A., Edmonds, J., & Stout, A. (2005). What learning to see arbitrary motion tells us about biological motion perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 1096–1106.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral Brain Science, 24, 849–937.Google Scholar
  18. Jastorff, J., Kourtzi, Z., & Giese, M. A. (2006). Learning to discriminate complex movements: biological versus artificial trajectories. Journal of Vision, 6, 791–804.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  20. Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception & Psychophysics, 14, 202–211.Google Scholar
  21. Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of actions: interactions of perception and action. Psychological Science, 2, 467–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210–220.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Maurer, D., Le Grand, R., & Mondloch, C. J. (2002). The many faces of configural processing. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 255–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Pavlova, M., & Sokolov, A. (2000). Orientation specificity in biological motion perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 889–899.Google Scholar
  25. Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Pyles, J. A., Garcia, J. O., Hoffman, D. D., & Grossman, E. D. (2007). Visual perception and neural correlates of novel ‘biological motion’. Vision Research, 47, 2786–2797.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Reed, C. L., Stone, V. E., Bozova, S., & Tanaka, J. (2003). The body-inversion effect. Psychological Science, 14, 302–308.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Shiffrar, M., & Pinto, J. (2002). The visual analysis of bodily motion. In W. Prinz, & B. Hommel (Eds.) Common mechanisms in perception and action: Attention and Performance (Vol. XIX, pp. 381–399). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 105(2), 809–813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Sumi, S. (1984). Upside-down presentation of the Johansson moving light-spot pattern. Perception, 13, 283–286.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Valentine, T. (1988). Upside-down faces—a review of the effect of inversion upon face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 471–491.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Yin, R. K. (1969). Looking at upside down faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 141–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Beatriz Calvo-Merino
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Shantel Ehrenberg
    • 4
    • 5
  • Delia Leung
    • 3
  • Patrick Haggard
    • 3
  1. 1.Psychology Department, School of Social ScienceCity University LondonLondonUK
  2. 2.Psychology DepartmentUniversidad Complutense de MadridMadridSpain
  3. 3.Institute of Cognitive NeuroscienceUniversity College LondonLondonUK
  4. 4.Faculty of Humanities, School of Languages, Linguistics and CulturesUniversity of ManchesterManchesterUK
  5. 5.Dance Science, Trinity LabanCreeksideLondonUK

Personalised recommendations