Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 73, Issue 2, pp 127–135 | Cite as

Saccadic eye movement programming: sensory and attentional factors

  • John M. FindlayEmail author
Original Article

Abstract

The relationship between attention and eye movements is explored by consideration of the variety of ways attention may affect saccade programming. Four forms of attention are distinguished: one exogenous and three endogenous through either locations or objects or features. Each of these can control or influence the saccadic generation process. Visual onsets generate a rapid transient signal with low resolution that travels from the visual system to the oculomotor system. Generation of an eye movement is associated with an attentional signal, probably also with low resolution, travelling from the oculomotor system to the visual system. A high-resolution attentional signal, which appears to require voluntary effort, can also select saccadic targets.

Keywords

Saccade Target Oculomotor System Covert Attention Voluntary Saccade Saccadic Movement 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Arai, K., McPeek, R. M., & Keller, E. (2004). Properties of saccadic responses in monkeys when competing visual stimuli are present. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 890–900.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baldauf, D., & Deubel, H. (2008). Properties of attentional selection during the preparation of saccade sequences. Experimental Brain Research, 184, 411–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Castet, E., Jeanjean, S., Montagnini, A., Laugier, D., & Masson, G. S. (2006). Dynamics of attentional deployment during saccade programming. Journal of Vision, 6, 196–212.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chou, I.-H., Sommer, M. A., & Schiller, P. H. (1999). Express averaging saccades in the monkey. Vision Research, 39, 4200–4216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Coëffé, C., & O’Regan, J. K. (1987). Reducing the influence of non-target stimuli on saccade accuracy: predictability and latency effects. Vision Research, 27, 227–240.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Deubel, H., Wolf, W., & Hauske, G. (1984). The evaluation of the oculomotor error signal. In A. G. Gale & F. Johnson (Eds.), Theoretical and applied aspects of eye movement research (pp. 55–62). North-Holland: Amsterdam.Google Scholar
  9. Dorris, M. C., & Munoz, D. P. (1995). A neural correlate for the gap effect on saccadic reaction times in monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 2558–2562.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Egly, R., Driver, J., & Rafal, R. D. (1994). Shifting visual attention between objects and locations: evidence from normal and parietal lesion subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 123, 161–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112, 777–813.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Eriksen, C. W., & St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention : a zoom lens model. Perception and Psychophysics, 40, 225–240.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Findlay, J. M. (1982). Global processing for saccadic eye movements. Vision Research, 22, 1033–1045.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Findlay, J. M. (1997). Saccade target selection in visual search. Vision Research, 37, 617–631.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Findlay, J. M. & Blythe, H. I. (2008). Saccade target selection: Do distractors affect saccade accuracy? Vision Research (submitted).Google Scholar
  16. Findlay, J. M., & Brown, V. (2006a). Eye scanning of multi-element displays I. Scanpath planning. Vision Research, 46, 179–195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Findlay, J. M., & Brown, V. (2006b). Eye scanning of multi-element displays II. Saccade planning. Vision Research, 46, 216–227.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Findlay, J. M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2003). Active Vision: the Psychology of Looking and Seeing (p. xiii+220). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Findlay, J. M., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2005). Eye guidance and visual search. In: G. Underwood (Ed.), Cognitive processes in eye guidance (pp. 259–281). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  20. Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccadic eye movement generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 661–721.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Fletcher-Watson, S., Findlay, J. M., Leekam, S. R., & Benson, V. (2008). Rapid detection of person information in a naturalistic scene. Perception, 37, 571–583.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2002). Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: Evidence for a competitive integration model. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1039–1054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Godijn, R., & Theeuwes, J. (2003). Parallel allocation of attention prior to the exection of saccade sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 29, 882–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hamker, F. H. (2004). A dynamic model of how feature cues guide spatial attention. Vision Research, 44, 501–521.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. He, P., & Kowler, E. (1989). The role of location probability in the programming of saccades: implications for “center-of-gravity” tendencies. Vision Research, 29, 1165–1181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. He, P., & Kowler, E. (1991). Saccade localizaton of eccentric forms. Journal of the Optical Society of America. Part A, 8, 440–449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Henderson, J. M. (2003). Human gaze control during real world scene perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 498–504.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual-attention in saccadic eye-movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Hollingworth, A., Richard, A. M., & Luck, S. J. (2008). Understanding the function of visual short-term memory: Transsaccadic memory, object correspondence, and gaze correction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 137, 163–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hyönä, J. (1995). Do irregular letter combinations attract readers’ attention: evidence from fixation locations in words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 21, 61–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Intriligator, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of visual attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 171–216.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kingstone, A., & Klein, R. M. (1993). Visual offset facilitates saccade latency: does pre-disengagement of attention mediate this gap effect ? Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance, 19, 1251–1261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kirchner, H., & Thorpe, S. J. (2006). Ultra-rapid object detection with saccadic eye movements: Visual processing speed revisited. Vision Research, 46, 1762–1776.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Klein, R., & Farrell, M. (1989). Search performance without eye-movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 46, 476–482.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Land, M. F., & Hayhoe, M. (2001). In what way do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? Vision Research, 41, 3559–3565.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. McSorley, E., Haggard, P., & Walker, R. (2005). Spatial and temporal aspects of oculomotor inhibition as revealed by saccadic trajectories. Vision Research, 45, 2492–2499.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Moore, T., & Fallah, M. (2001). Control of eye movements and spatial attention. Proceedings of the National Academy Science USA, 98, 1273–1276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Moore, T., & Fallah, M. (2004). Microstimulation of the frontal eye field and its effects on covert spatial attention. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91, 152–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Motter, B. C., & Belky, E. J. (1998). The guidance of eye movements during active visual search. Vision Research, 38, 1805–1815.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Motter, B. C., & Holsapple, J. (2007). Saccades and covert shifts of attention during active visual search: Spatial distributions, memory and items per fixation. Vision Research, 47, 1261–1281.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Peterson, M. S., Kramer, A. F., & Irwin, D. E. (2004). Covert shifts of attention precede involuntary eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 66, 398–405.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. F., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105, 125–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltà, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians : evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsycholgia, 25, 31–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 57, 1030–1033.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Schall, J. D., & Hanes, D. P. (1993). Neural basis of target selection in frontal eye field during visual search. Nature, 366, 467–469.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Schall, J. D., & Thompson, K. G. (1999). Neural selection and control of visually guided eye movements. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 241–259.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Schneider, W. X. (1995). VAM: Neuro-cognitive model for visual attention control of segmentation, object recognition, and space-based motor action. Visual Cognition, 2, 331–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Schneider, W. X., & Deubel, H. (2002). Selection-for-perception and selection-for-spatial motor-action are coupled by visual attention: a review of recent findings and new evidence from stimulus-driven saccade control. In W. Prinz & B. Hommel (Eds.), Attention and performance XIX: common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 609–627). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  51. Sheliga, B. M., Riggio, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1994). Orienting of attention and eye movements. Experimental Brain Research, 98, 507–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Shepherd, M., Findlay, J. M., & Hockey, G. R. J. (1986). The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 475–491.Google Scholar
  53. Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., & Irwin, D. E. (1998). Our eyes do not always go where we want them to go: Capture of the eyes by new objects. Psychological Science, 9, 379–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Vergilino-Perez, D., & Findlay, J. M. (2004). Object structure and saccade planning. Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 525–528.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Walker, R., Deubel, H., Schneider, W. X., & Findlay, J. M. (1997). The effect of remote distractors on saccade programming : evidence for an extended fixation zone. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 1108–1119.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Walker, R., Kentridge, R. W., & Findlay, J. M. (1995). Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies. Experimental Brain Research, 103, 294–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. White, S. J., & Liversedge, S. P. (2006). Linguistic and nonlinguistic influences on the eyes’ landing positions during reading. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 760–782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Williams, D. E., Reingold, E. M., Moscovitch, M., & Behrmann, M. (1997). Patterns of eye movements during parallel and serial visual search tasks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Experimentale, 51, 151–164.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Wolfe, J. M. (1998). What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search? Psychological Science, 9, 33–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wong, J. H., Peterson, M. S., & Hillstrom, A. P. (2007). Are changes in semantic and structural information sufficient for oculomotor capture? Journal of Vision, 7, 1–10.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of DurhamDurhamUK

Personalised recommendations