Psychological Research

, Volume 72, Issue 6, pp 657–665

The initial capitalization superiority effect in German: evidence for a perceptual frequency variant of the orthographic cue hypothesis of visual word recognition

  • Arthur M. Jacobs
  • Hans-Christoph Nuerk
  • Ralf Graf
  • Mario Braun
  • Tatjana A. Nazir
Original Article

Abstract

A perceptual frequency variant of the orthographic cue (OC) hypothesis (Peressotti, Cubelli, & Job, 2003) was tested in two perceptual identification experiments using the variable viewing position technique: German nouns and non-nouns that are most frequently perceived with or without initial letter capitalization, respectively, were tachistoscopically presented in upper-case, lower-case, or with initial capitalization. The results indicated that words were best recognized in the form they are most frequently perceived in, which suggests that during reading acquisition abstract as well as case- and item-specific OCs may be learned and used for recognition.

References

  1. Aghababian, V., & Nazir, T. A. (2000). Developing normal reading skills: Aspects of visual processes underlying word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 76, 123–150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baayen R. H., Piepenbrock R., & van Rijn H. (1993). The CELEX Lexical Database (CDROM). Philadelphia: Lingusitic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
  3. Besner, D., Davelaar, E., Alcott, D., & Parry, P. (1984). Wholistic reading of alphabetic print: Evidence from FBM and FBI. In L. Henderson (Ed.), Orthographies and reading (pp. 121–135). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  4. Bock, M. (1989). Lesen in Abhängigkeit von der Groß-und Kleinschreibung (The dependency of reading on capital and small letters). Sprache & Kognition, 8, 133–151.Google Scholar
  5. Bock, M., Augst, G., & Wegner, I. (1985). Gross oder klein? Zur Funktion des Wortanfangs für den gegenwärtigen Leser (The function of capital letters and small letters for present-day readers). Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 17(3), 191–209.Google Scholar
  6. Bouma, H., & Legein, C. P. (1977). Foveal and parafoveal recognition of letters and words by dyslexics and by average readers. Neuropsychologia, 15, 69–80.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brysbaert, M., & Nazir, T. (2005). Visual constraints in written word recognition: Evidence from the optimal viewing-position effect. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(3), 216–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brysbaert, M., Vitu, F., & Schroyens, W. (1996). The right visual field advantage and the optimal viewing position effect: On the relation between foveal and parafoveal word recognition. Neuropsychology, 10, 385–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 335–341.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Farid, M., & Grainger, J. (1996). How initial fixation position influences visual word recognition: A comparison of French and Arabic. Brain and Language, 53, 351–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Henderson, L., & Chard, M. J. (1976). On the nature of the facilitation of visual comparisons by lexical membership. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 7, 432–434.Google Scholar
  13. Hillis, A. E., Newhart, M., Heidler, J., Barker, P., Herskovits, E., & Degaonkar, M. (2005). The roles of the visual word form area in reading. NeuroImage, 24, 548–559.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Jacobs, A. M., & Graf, R. (2005). Wortformgedächtnis als intuitive Statistik in Sprachen mit unterschiedlicher Konsistenz. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 213, 133–141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Jacobs, A. M., Graf, R., & Kinder, A. (2003). Receiver-operating characteristics in the lexical decision task: Evidence for a simple signal detection process simulated by the multiple read-out model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory and cognition, 29, 481–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German-English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315–334.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Mayall, K., & Humphreys, G. W. (1996). Case mixing and the task sensitive disruption of lexical processing. Journal of Experimental psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22, 278–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 293–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. W., Reddix, M. D., Zola, D., & Jacobs, A. M. (1989). Eye movement control during reading: II. Frequency of refixating a word. Perception and Psychophysics, 46, 245–253.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Montant, M., Nazir, T. A., & Poncet, M. (1998). Pure alexia and the viewing position effect in printed words. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 93–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Musseler, J., Nisslein, M., & Koriat, A. (2005). German capitalization of nouns and the detection of letters in continuous text. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 143–158.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Nazir, T. A., Heller, D., & Sussmann, C. (1992). Letter visibility and word recognition: the optimal viewing position in printed words. Perception and Psychophysics, 52, 315–328.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Nazir, T. A., Jacobs, A. M., & O’Regan, J. K. (1998). Letter legibility and visual word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 26, 810–821.Google Scholar
  24. Nazir, T. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Jacobs, A. M. (1991). On words and their letters. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 171–174.Google Scholar
  25. Nuerk, H.-C. (2001). On the importance of visual determinants in visual word recognition. Ph.D. Thesis, Philipps-Universität Marburg, Marburg, Germany.Google Scholar
  26. O’Regan, J. K., & Jacobs, A. M. (1992). Optimal viewing position effect in word recognition: A challenge to current theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 185–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. O’Regan, J. K., Levy-Schoen, A., Pynte, J., & Brugaillere, B. (1984). Convenient fixation location within isolated words of different length and structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 250–257.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Oßwald, K., Brustmann, D., Nazir, T., Nuerk, H.-C., Graf, R., & Jacobs, A. M. (1998). Die optimale Blickposition bei lese-rechtschreibschwachen Kindern. In H. Lachnit, A. Jacobs & F. Rösler (Eds.), Experimentelle Psychologie: Abstracts der 40 Tagung experimentell arbeitender Psychologen (p. 252). Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.Google Scholar
  29. Paap, K. R., Newsome, S. L., & Noel, R. W. (1984). Word shapes in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 413–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Peressotti, F., Cubelli, R., & Job, R. (2003). On recognizing proper names: The orthographic cue hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 87–116.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Underwood, G., Clews, S., & Everatt, J. (1990). How do readers know where to look next? Local information distributions influence eye fixations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology [A], 42, 39–65.Google Scholar
  32. Vitu, F., O’Regan, J. K., & Mittau, M. (1990). Optimal landing position in reading isolated words and continuous text. Perception and Psychophysics, 47, 583–600.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English children. Cognition, 51, 91–103.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., & Braun, M. (2001). Identical words are read differently in different languages. Psychological Science, 27, 379–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Arthur M. Jacobs
    • 1
  • Hans-Christoph Nuerk
    • 2
  • Ralf Graf
    • 3
  • Mario Braun
    • 1
  • Tatjana A. Nazir
    • 4
  1. 1.Arbeitsbereich Allgemeine und Neurokognitive Psychologie, Fachbereich Erziehungswissenschaft und PsychologieFreie Universität BerlinBerlinGermany
  2. 2.Universität TübingenTübingenGermany
  3. 3.Katholische Universität Eichstätt-IngolstadtEichstättGermany
  4. 4.Institut des Sciences Cognitives, CNRSLyonFrance

Personalised recommendations