Psychological Research

, Volume 70, Issue 3, pp 200–217 | Cite as

Organization of visuo-spatial serial memory: interaction of temporal order with spatial and temporal grouping

  • Fabrice B. R. Parmentier
  • Pilar Andrés
  • Greg Elford
  • Dylan M. Jones
Original Article

Abstract

This study investigates whether memory for sequences of spatial locations can be represented hierarchically, that is, as successive groups containing the order of constituent locations. Two grouping manipulations are used: Temporal grouping, based on the verbal serial memory literature, and spatial grouping, based on recent empirical work on visuo-spatial serial memory. In Experiment 1, we examine the relationship between spatial grouping and temporal order and showed that recall performance increases when both temporal and spatial organization correlate, but decreases when they clash. Experiments 2 and 3 show that the latter result is confounded by differences in path length (length of spatial path defined by the locations) between conditions, and that no effect of the spatial organization is observed when path length is controlled for. In Experiment 4, an alternative method to spatial grouping, temporal grouping, is used to induce hierarchical organization. A recall advantage is found in the temporal grouping condition. The results suggest that hierarchical representations can be imposed on order information for visuo-spatial sequences, either when participants have pre-existing knowledge about the form of the path formed by the sequence or when temporal boundaries delimit chunks; that increased path length is the cause of the performance decrement observed when dots from separate spatial groups are presented successively; and that path length and more generally sequence characteristics should be taken into account in designing future research on visuo-spatial serial memory.

Notes

Acknowledgements

Fabrice B.R. Parmentier, Pilar Andrés, Greg Elford. School of Psychology, University of Plymouth, UK. Dylan M. Jones, School of Psychology, Cardiff University, UK, and School of Psychology, University of Western Australia. Thanks are due to Marie Delhez for collecting some of the data. This research was supported by a research grant (RES-000-22-0233) from the Economic and Social Research Council (E.S.R.C.), UK. We also thank Marc Brysbaert and two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments on an earlier version of this article.

References

  1. Anderson, J. R., & Matessa, M. (1997). A production system theory of serial memory. Psychological Review, 104, 728–748.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Lebiere, C., & Matessa, M. (1998). An integrated theory of list memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 341–380.Google Scholar
  3. Avons, S. E. (1998). Serial report and item recognition of novel visual patterns. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 285–308.Google Scholar
  4. Awh, E., Jonides, J., Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (1998). Rehearsal in spatial working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 780–790.Google Scholar
  5. Awh, E., Jonides, J., Smith, E.E., Buxton, R.B., Frank, L.R., Love, T., Wong, E.C., & Gmeindl, L. (1999). Rehearsal in spatial working memory: Evidence from neuroimaging. Psychological Science, 10, 433–437.Google Scholar
  6. Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
  7. Berch, D. B., Krikorian, R., & Huha, E. M. (1998). The Corsi-tapping task: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Brain and Cognition, 38, 317–338.Google Scholar
  8. Bower, G. H., & Springston, F. (1970). Pauses as recoding points in letter series. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 421–430.Google Scholar
  9. Bower, G. H., & Winzenz, D. (1969). Group structure, coding, and memory for digits series. Journal of Experimental Psychology Monograph Supplement, 80, 1–17.Google Scholar
  10. Broadbent, D. E., & Broadbent, M. H. P. (1981). Recency effects in visual memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 1–15.Google Scholar
  11. Brown, G. D. A., Preece, T., & Hulme, C. (2000). Oscillator-based memory for serial order. Psychological Review, 107, 127–181.Google Scholar
  12. Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. (1992). Toward a network model of the articulatory loop. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 429–460.Google Scholar
  13. Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. (1996). A connectionist model of STM for serial order. In S. E. Gathercole (Ed.), Models of short-term memory (pp. 51–72). Hove, UK: Psychology.Google Scholar
  14. Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. Psychological Review, 106, 551–581.Google Scholar
  15. Compton, B. J., & Logan, G. D. (1993). Evaluating a computational model of perceptual grouping by proximity. Perception and Psychophysics, 53, 403–421.Google Scholar
  16. Compton, B. J., & Logan, G. D. (1999). Judgments of perceptual groups: Reliability and sensitivity to stimulus transformation. Perception & Psychophysics, 61, 1320–1335.Google Scholar
  17. Cooper, R., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297–338.Google Scholar
  18. Couture, M., & Tremblay, S. (in press). A Hebb effect for visuo-spatial information. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  19. Crowder, R. G. (1976). Principles of learning and memory. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. De Lillo, C. (2004). Imposing structure on a Corsi-type task: Evidence for hierarchical organisation based on spatial proximity in serial-spatial memory. Brain and Cognition, 55, 415–426.Google Scholar
  21. De Ribaupierre, A., & Bailleux, C. (1994). Developmental change in a spatial task of attentional capacity: An essay toward an integration of two working memory models. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 17, 5–35.Google Scholar
  22. Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Uber das Gedächtnis. Untersuchungen zur experimentellen Psychologie. Leipzig, Germany: Duncker & Humblot.Google Scholar
  23. Estes, W. K. (1972). An associative basis for coding and organization in memory. In: A. W. Melton, E. Martin (Eds.), Coding processes in human memory (pp. 161–190). Washington: Winston.Google Scholar
  24. Farah, M. J., Hammond, K. M., Levine, D. N., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439–462.Google Scholar
  25. Farrand, P., & Jones, D. M. (1996). Direction of report in spatial and verbal serial short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 140–158.Google Scholar
  26. Farrand, P., Parmentier, F. B. R., & Jones, D. M. (2001). Temporal-spatial memory: Retrieval of spatial information does not reduce recency. Acta Psychologica, 106, 285–301.Google Scholar
  27. Frankish, C. (1985). Modality-specific grouping effects in short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 381–389.Google Scholar
  28. Frankish, C. (1989). Perceptual organization and PAS. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 15, 469–479.Google Scholar
  29. Frankish, C. (1995). Intonation and auditory grouping in immediate serial recall. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 9, S5–S22.Google Scholar
  30. Fusch, A. (1969). Recall for order and content of serial word lists in short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 14–21.Google Scholar
  31. Hälbig, T. D., Mecklinger, A., Schriefers, H., & Friederich, A. D. (1998). Double dissociation of processing temporal and spatial information in working memory. Neuropsychologia, 36, 305–311.Google Scholar
  32. Hale, S., Myerson, J., Rhee, S. H., Weiss, C. S., & Abrams, R. A. (1996). Selective interference with the maintenance of location information in working memory. Neuropsychology, 2, 228–240.Google Scholar
  33. Healy, A. F. (1974). Separating item from order information in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 644–655.Google Scholar
  34. Henson, R. N. A. (1996). Short-term memory for serial order. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
  35. Henson, R. N. A. (1998). Short-term memory for serial order: The start-end model. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 73–137.Google Scholar
  36. Hitch, G. J., Burgess, N., Towse, J. N., & Culpin, V. (1996) Temporal grouping in immediate recall: A working memory analysis. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 116–139.Google Scholar
  37. Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development of short-term memory span. Journal of Memory & Language, 30, 685–701.Google Scholar
  38. Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Brown, G., & Mercer, R. (1995). The role of long-term memory mechanisms in memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 527–536.Google Scholar
  39. Jensen, A. R. (1962). Temporal and spatial effects of serial position. American Journal of Psychology, 75, 390–400.Google Scholar
  40. Johnson, N. F. (1970). The role of chunking and organization in the process of recall. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 4 (pp. 171–247). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  41. Jones, D., Farrand, P., Stuart, G., & Morris, N. (1995). Functional equivalence of verbal and spatial information in serial short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1008–1018.Google Scholar
  42. Kahana, M. J., & Jacobs, J. (2000). Interresponse time in serial recall: Effect of intraserial repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology; Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1188–1197.Google Scholar
  43. Katz, D. (1950). Gestalt psychology. New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
  44. Kessels, R. P. C., van Zandvoort, K. J. E., Postma, A., Kappelle, L. J., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2000). The Corsi block-tapping task: Standardization and normative data. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 252–258.Google Scholar
  45. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
  46. Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. The Hixon symposium (pp. 112–136). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  47. Lawrence, B. M., Myerson, J., Oonk, H. M., & Abrams, R. A. (2001). The effects of eye and limb movements on working memory. Memory, 9, 433–444.Google Scholar
  48. Lawrence, B. M., Myerson, J., Abrams, R. A. (2004). Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 488–494.Google Scholar
  49. Lee, C. L., & Estes, W. K. (1977). Order and position in primary memory for letter strings. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 395–418.Google Scholar
  50. Lee, C. L., & Estes, W. K. (1981). Item and order information in short-term memory: Evidence for multilevel perturbation processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 7, 149–169.Google Scholar
  51. Lewandowsky, S., & Farrell, S. (2000). A redintegration account of the effects of speech rate, lexicality, and word frequency in immediate serial recall. Psychological Research, 63, 163–173.Google Scholar
  52. Magnussen, S., Dyrnes, S., & Nordy, K. (1997). Micro-position effects in visual short-term memory. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 139–142.Google Scholar
  53. Marshuetz, C., Smith, E. E., Jonides, J., DeGutis, J., Chenevert, T. L. (2000). Order information in working memory: fMRI evidence for parietal and prefrontal mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 130–144.Google Scholar
  54. Maybery, M. T., Parmentier, F. B. R., & Jones, D. M. (2002). Grouping of list items reflected in the timing of recall: Implications for models of serial verbal memory. Journal of Memory & Language, 47, 360–385.Google Scholar
  55. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex—insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review, 102, 419–457.Google Scholar
  56. Nairne, J. S. (1992). The loss of positional certainty in long-term memory. Psychological Science, 3, 199–202.Google Scholar
  57. Neath, I. (1997). Modality, concreteness, and set-size effects in a free reconstruction of order task. Memory and Cognition, 25, 256–263.Google Scholar
  58. Ng, H. L. H., & Maybery, M. T. (2002). Temporal grouping effects in short-term memory: An evaluation of time-dependent models. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 391–424.Google Scholar
  59. Page, M.P.A., & Norris, D. (1998a). The primacy model: A new model of immediate serial recall. Psychological Review, 105, 761–781.Google Scholar
  60. Page, M. P. A., & Norris, D. (1998b). Modeling immediate serial recall with a localist implementation of the primacy model. In J. Grainger, A. M. Jacobs (Eds.), Localist connectionist approaches to human cognition (pp. 227–255). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  61. Parmentier, F. B. R., & Jones, D. M. (2000). Functional characteristics of auditory temporal-spatial short-term memory: Evidence from serial order errors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 222–238.Google Scholar
  62. Parmentier, F. B. R., Maybery, M. T., & Jones, D. M. (2004). Temporal grouping in auditory spatial serial memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 501–507.Google Scholar
  63. Parmentier, F. B. R., Tremblay, S., & Jones, D. M. (2004). Exploring the suffix effect in serial visuo-spatial short-term memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 289–295.Google Scholar
  64. Parmentier, F. B. R., Elford, G., & Maybery, M. T. (in press). Transitional information in visuo-spatial serial memory: Path characteristics affect recall performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition.Google Scholar
  65. Pearson, D. G., & Sahraie, A. (2003). Oculomotor control and maintenance of spatially and temporally distributed events in visuo-spatial working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 1089–1111.Google Scholar
  66. Phillips, W. A., & Christie, D. F. (1977a). Components of visual memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 117–133.Google Scholar
  67. Phillips, W. A., & Christie, D. F. (1977b). Interference with visualization. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 637–650.Google Scholar
  68. Quinn, J. G., & Ralston, G. E. (1986). Movement and attention in visual working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 689–703.Google Scholar
  69. Roodernys, S., & Quinlan, P. T. (2000). The effects of stimulus set size and word frequency on verbal serial recall. Memory, 8, 71–78.Google Scholar
  70. Ryan, J. (1969a). Grouping and short-term memory: Different means and patterns of grouping. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 137–147.Google Scholar
  71. Ryan, J. (1969b). Temporal grouping, rehearsal and short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 148–155.Google Scholar
  72. Saito, S. (1998). Effects of articulatory suppression on immediate serial recall of temporally grouped and intonated lists. Psychologia, 41, 95–101.Google Scholar
  73. Schuman-Hengsteler, R., Strobl, M., & Zoelch, C. (2004). Temporal memory for locations: On the coding of spatiotemporal information in children and adults. In G. L. Allen (Ed.), Human spatial memory. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  74. Sheffield, F. D. (1961). Theoretical considerations in the learning of complex sequential tasks from demonstrations and practice. In A. A. Lumsdaine (Ed.), Student response in programmed instruction, Publication 943 (pp 13–32). Washington: National Academy of Sciences/National Research Council, Washington.Google Scholar
  75. Shimamura, A. P., Janowsky, J. S., & Squire, L. R. (1990). Memory for temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 28, 803–813.Google Scholar
  76. Smyth, M. M. (1996). Interference with rehearsal in spatial working memory in the absence of eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Journal, 49A, 940–949.Google Scholar
  77. Smyth, M. M., & Scholey, K. A. (1992). Determining spatial span: The role of movement time and articulation rate. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45A, 479–501.Google Scholar
  78. Smyth, M. M., & Scholey, K. A. (1994). Characteristics of spatial memory span: Is there an analogy to the word length effect based on movement time? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 91–117.Google Scholar
  79. Smyth, M. M., & Scholey, K. A. (1996). Serial order in spatial immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 159–177.Google Scholar
  80. Smyth, M. M., Pearson, N. A., & Pendleton, L. R. (1988). Movement and working memory: Patterns and positions in space. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 497–514.Google Scholar
  81. Stuart, G., & Hulme, C. (2000). The effects of word co-occurrence on short-term memory: Associative links in long-term memory affects short-term memory performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 796–802.Google Scholar
  82. Tremblay, S., Macken, W. J., & Jones, D. M. (2001). The impact of broadband noise on serial memory: Changes in band-pass frequency increase disruption. Memory, 9, 323–331.Google Scholar
  83. Tremblay, S., Nicholls, A., Parmentier, F. B. R., & Jones, D. M. (in press). Visual distraction and visuo-spatial memory: A sandwich effect. Memory.Google Scholar
  84. Ward, G., Avons, S. E., & Melling, L. (in press). Serial position curves in short-term memory: Functional equivalence across modalities. Memory.Google Scholar
  85. Zimmer, H. D., Speiser, H. R., & Seidler, B. (2003). Spatio-temporal working-memory and short-term object-location tasks use different memory mechanisms. Acta Psychologica, 114, 41–65.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2005

Authors and Affiliations

  • Fabrice B. R. Parmentier
    • 1
  • Pilar Andrés
    • 1
  • Greg Elford
    • 1
  • Dylan M. Jones
    • 2
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of PlymouthDevonUK
  2. 2.School of PsychologyCardiff UniversityCardiffUK
  3. 3.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Western AustraliaCrawleyAustralia

Personalised recommendations