Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 69, Issue 1–2, pp 67–76 | Cite as

Attentional selection during preparation of eye movements

  • Karine Doré-Mazars
  • Pierre Pouget
  • Cécile Beauvillain
Original Article

Abstract

Two experiments were conducted to examine the relationship between visual attention and saccade programming. Participants had to saccade to a letter string and detect a letter change presented briefly before the saccade onset. Hit probability (i.e., correct detection of a letter change in different positions) was taken as a measure of visual attention focus. The first experiment shows that hit probability depends on the actual landing position. These findings argue for a spatial coupling between saccade programming and the orienting of attention. Also, an unfamiliar letter cluster at the beginning captures attention and prevails over the influence of the saccade in preparation. Experiment 2, in which the letter change occurred at different times during the saccade latency, shows that attention shifts and focuses on the saccade target at the expense of the other parts of the stimulus when the motor program is ready to be executed. The theoretical implications of these results are discussed.

Keywords

Letter String Saccade Latency Saccade Target Landing Position Saccade Goal 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by grant 5973272 from the Institut National de la Recherche et de la Sécurité. Experiment 2 is part of P. Pouget’s ongoing doctoral thesis at the Université René Descartes Paris 5. We thank Heiner Deubel, Keith Rayner and Thérèse Collins for helpful comments on an earlier version of the paper.

References

  1. Beauvillain, C., & Beauvillain, P. (1995). Calibration of an eye movement system for use in reading. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 27, 331–337.Google Scholar
  2. Beauvillain, C., & Doré, K. (1998). Orthographic codes are used in integrating information from the parafovea by the saccadic computation system. Vision Research, 38, 115–123.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Beauvillain, C., Doré, K., & Baudouin, V. (1996). The “center of gravity” of words: Evidence for an effect of the word-initial letters. Vision Research, 36, 589–603.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Umilta, C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–1692.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Currie, C. B., McConkie, G. W., Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Irwin, D. E. (2000). The role of the saccade target object in the perception of a visually stable word. Perception and Psychophysics, 62, 673–683.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Deubel, H., & Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vision Research, 36, 1827–1837.Google Scholar
  7. Doré, K., & Beauvillain, C. (1997). Latency dependence of word-initial letter integration by the saccadic system. Perception and Psychophysics, 59, 523–533.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Findlay, J. M., & Walker, R. (1999). A model of saccade generation based on parallel processing and competitive inhibition. Behavioral and Brain Science, 78, 1108–1119.Google Scholar
  9. Fischer M. H. (1999). An investigation of attention allocation during sequential eye movement tasks. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 649–677.Google Scholar
  10. Henderson, J. M. (1993). Visual attention and saccadic eye movements. In G. d’Ydewalle & J. Van Rensbergen (Eds.), Perception and Cognition (pp. 37–50). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  11. Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The role of fixation position in detecting scene changes across saccades. Psychological Science, 10, 438–443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Henderson, J. M., Pollatsek, A., & Rayner, K. (1989). Covert visual attention and extrafoveal information use during object identification. Perception and Psychophysics, 45, 196–208.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Henderson, J. M., Weeks, P. A., & Hollingworth, A. (1999). The effects of semantic consistency on eye movements during complex scene viewing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 210–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hoffman, J. E., & Subramaniam, B. (1995) The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 787–795.Google Scholar
  15. Hyönä, J. (1995). Do irregular letter combinations attract reader’s attention? Evidence from fixation locations in words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 68–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Inhoff, A. W., Pollatsek, A, Posner, M. I., & Rayner, K. (1989). Covert attention and eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41A, 63–89.Google Scholar
  17. Irwin, D. E., Colcombe, A. M., Kramer, A. F., & Hahn, S. (2000). Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons. Vision Research, 40, 1443–1458.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Itti, L., & Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research, 40, 1489–1506.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Kim, M. S., & Cave, K. R. (1999). Top-down and bottom-up attentional control: On the nature of interference from a salient distractor, Perception and Psychophysics, 61, 1009–1023.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Klein, R. (1980). Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? In R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 259–276). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Klein, R., Kingstone, A., & Pontefract, A. (1992). Orienting of visual attention. In K. Rayner (Ed.), Eye movements and visual cognition: Scene perception and reading (pp. 46–65). New York Berlin Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  22. Koch, C., & Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in visual attention: Toward the underlying circuitry. Human Neurobiology, 4, 219–227.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B., & Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vision Research, 35, 1897–1916.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Laberge, D., & Brown, V. (1989). Theory of attentional operations in shape identification. Psychological Review, 96, 101–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. McConkie, G. W., & Currie, C. B. (1996). Visual stability across saccades while viewing complex pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 563–581.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. McConkie, G. W., Kerr, P. W., Reddix, M. D., & Zola, D. (1988). Eye movement control during reading: The location of initial eye fixations on words. Vision Research, 27, 227–240.Google Scholar
  27. O’Regan, J. K. (1979). Saccade size control in reading: Evidence for the linguistic control hypothesis. Perception and Psychophysics, 25, 501–509.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Rayner, K. (1979). Eye guidance in reading: Fixation location within words. Perception, 8, 21–30.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1987) Eye movements in reading: A tutorial review. In K. Rayner (Ed.), Eye movements in reading. Perceptual and language processes (pp. 327–362). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  30. Rayner, K., & Pollatsek, A. (1992). Eye movements and scene perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 342–376.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Remington R. W. (1980). Attention and saccadic eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6, 726–744.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umilta, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the vertical and horizontal meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Shepherd, M., Findlay, J. M., & Hockey, R. J. (1986). The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 475–491.Google Scholar
  34. Theeuwes, J., Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Irwin, D. E., & Zelinky, G. J. (1999) Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1595–1608.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Theeuwes, J, Atchley, P., & Kramer, A. F. (2000). On the time course of top-down and bottom-up control of visual attention. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Attention and Performance XVIII. Cambridge, MA: MIT.Google Scholar
  36. Trésor de la Langue Française (1971). Dictionnaire des fréquences. Paris: Klincksieck.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2004

Authors and Affiliations

  • Karine Doré-Mazars
    • 1
  • Pierre Pouget
    • 1
  • Cécile Beauvillain
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratoire de Psychologie ExpérimentaleUMR 8581 CNRS/Université René Descartes Paris VBoulogne-Billancourt CedexFrance

Personalised recommendations