Advertisement

Psychological Research

, Volume 69, Issue 1–2, pp 1–10 | Cite as

Memory for actions: Item and relational information in categorized lists

  • Johannes Engelkamp
  • Kerstin H. Seiler
  • Hubert D. Zimmer
Original Article

Abstract

Enacting action phrases in subject-performed tasks (SPTs) leads to better free recall than hearing or reading the same materials in verbal tasks (VTs). This enactment effect is usually explained by better item-specific information in SPTs than in VTs. The role of relational information is controversial. In the present paper, we will take the multiple recall approach to study the role of item and relational information in memory for actions by computing the number of item gains and the number of item losses over trials. This approach has previously been applied to lists of unrelated action phrases. We applied it to categorically related lists, also allowing a measure of relational information by clustering scores. It was found that SPTs produced more item gains than VTs. This finding confirmed the assumption that SPTs provide better item-specific information than VTs. The number of item losses did not differ between VTs and SPTs. This finding suggests that relational information is equally provided by VTs and SPTs. However, the organizational scores showed a more differentiated picture. Clustering was greater in SPTs than in VTs with randomly presented lists, but not with blocked lists. We suggested that these results, as well as other findings from the literature, could be explained by distinguishing automatic and strategic processes and the types of item associations that are addressed by these processes.

Keywords

Free Recall Recall Test Strategic Process Recall Trial List Structure 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (En 124/13). We would like to thank Nicola Ferdinand, Jochen Glössner, Hedda Janssen and Annabelle Saffran for their assistance with data collection.

References

  1. Anderson, J. R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295.Google Scholar
  2. Bäckman, L., Nilsson, L. G., & Chalom, D. (1986). New evidence on the nature of the encoding of action events. Memory & Cognition, 14, 339–346.Google Scholar
  3. Bousfield, W. A. (1953). The occurrence of clustering in the recall of randomly arranged associates. Journal of General Psychology, 49, 229–240.Google Scholar
  4. Burns, D. J. (1993). Item gains and item losses during hypermnesic recall: Implications for the item-specific-relational information distinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 163–173.Google Scholar
  5. Burns, D. J., & Gold, D. E. (1999). An analysis of item gains and losses in retroactive interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 978–985.Google Scholar
  6. Burns, D. J., & Schoff, K. M. (1998). Slow and steady often ties the race: Effects of item-specific and relational processing on cumulative recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1041–1051.Google Scholar
  7. Cohen, R. L. (1981). On the generality of some memory laws. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 22, 267–281.Google Scholar
  8. Cohen, R. L. (1983). The effect of encoding variables on the free recall of words and action events. Memory & Cognition, 11, 575–582.Google Scholar
  9. Cohen, R. L. (1985). On the generality of the laws of memory. In L. G. Nilsson & T. Archer (Eds.), Animal learning and human memory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  10. Einstein, G. O., & Hunt, R. R. (1980). Levels of processing and organization: Additive effects of individual item and relational processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 588–598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Engelkamp, J. (1986). Nouns and verbs in paired-associate learning: Instructional effects. Psychological Research, 48, 153–159.Google Scholar
  12. Engelkamp, J. (1988). Modality-specific encoding and word class in verbal learning. In M. Gruneberg, P. Morris, & R. N. Snykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (Vol. 1, pp. 415–420). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
  13. Engelkamp, J. (1995). Visual imagery and enactment in memory of actions. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 227–240.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for actions. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  15. Engelkamp, J. & Seiler, K. (2003). Gains and losses in action memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 56A, 829–848.Google Scholar
  16. Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1996). Organization and recall in verbal tasks and in subject-performed tasks. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 257–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (2002). Free recall and organization as a function of varying relational encoding in action memory. Psychological Research, 66, 91–98.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Engelkamp, J., Mohr, G., & Zimmer, H. D. (1991). Pair-relational encoding of performed nouns and verbs. Psychological Research, 53, 232–239.Google Scholar
  19. Engelkamp, J., Jahn, P., & Seiler, K. H. (2003). The item-order hypothesis reconsidered: The role of order information in free recall. Psychological Research, 67, 280–290.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Engelkamp, J., Seiler, K. H., & Zimmer, H. D. (2003). Differential relational encoding of categorical information in memory for action events. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  21. Gollin, E. S., & Sharps, M. J. (1988). Facilitation of free recall by categorical blocking depends on stimulus type. Memory & Cognition, 16, 539–544.Google Scholar
  22. Golly-Häring, C., & Engelkamp, J. (2003). Categorical-relational and order-relational information in memory for subject-performed and experimenter-performed actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 965–975.Google Scholar
  23. Helstrup, T. (1987). One, two or three memories? A problem-solving approach to memory for performed acts. Acta Psychologica, 66, 37–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hunt, R. R., & Einstein, G. O. (1981). Relational and item-specific information in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 497–514.Google Scholar
  25. Hunt, R. R., & McDaniel, M. A. (1993). The enigma of organization and distinctiveness. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 421–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., Kihlstrom, J. F., & Aseron, R. (1989). Effects of item-specific and relational information on hypermnesic recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 1192–1197.Google Scholar
  27. Knopf, M. (1991). Having shaved a kiwi fruit: Memory of unfamiliar subject-performed actions. Psychological Research, 53, 203–565.Google Scholar
  28. Koriat, A., & Pearlman-Avnion, S. (2003). Memory organization of action events and its relationship to memory performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 435–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kormi-Nouri, R. (1995). The nature of memory for action events: An episodic integration view. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 337–363.Google Scholar
  30. MacLeod, C. M., & Bassili, J. N. (1989). Are implicit and explicit tests differentially sensitive to item-specific versus relational information? In S. Lewandowsky & J. C. Dunn (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues (pp. 159–172). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  31. Mandler, J. M. (1979). Categorical and schematic organization in memory. In C. R. Puff (Ed.) Memory, organization and structure (pp. 303–317). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  32. McDaniel, M. A., Moore, B. A., & Whiteman, H. L. (1998). Dynamic changes in hypermnesia across early and late tests: A relational/item-specific account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 173–185.Google Scholar
  33. McDaniel, M. A., DeLosh, E. L., & Merritt, P. (2000). Order information and retrieval distinctiveness: Recall of common versus bizarre material. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 422–435.Google Scholar
  34. McDaniel, M. A., Einstein, G. O., Dunay, P. K., & Cobb, R. E. (1986). Encoding difficulty and memory: Toward a unifying theory. Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 645–656.Google Scholar
  35. Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Mohr, G., Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1989). Recall and recognition of self-performed acts. Psychological Research, 51, 181–187.Google Scholar
  37. Mulligan, N. (2000). Perceptual interference at encoding enhances item-specific encoding and disrupts relational encoding: Evidence from multiple recall tests. Memory & Cognition, 28, 539–546.Google Scholar
  38. Mulligan, N. (2001). Generation and hypermnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 436–450.Google Scholar
  39. Nairne, J. S., Pusen, C., & Widener, R. J., Jr. (1985). Representation in the mental lexicon: Implications for theories of the generation effect. Memory & Cognition, 13, 183–191.Google Scholar
  40. Nairne, J. S., Riegler, G. L., & Serra, M. (1991). Dissociative effects of generation on item and order retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 702–709.Google Scholar
  41. Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 106, 226–254.Google Scholar
  42. Nilsson, L. G., Nyberg, L., Klingberg, F., Åberg, C., Persson, J., & Roland, P. E. (2000). Activity in motor areas while remembering action events. Neuro Report, 11, 2199–2201.Google Scholar
  43. Norris, M., & West, R. (1993). Activity memory and aging: The role of motor retrieval and strategic processing. Psychology and Aging, 8, 81–86.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Nyberg, L., Petersson, K. M., Nilsson, L. G., Sandblom, J., Åberg, C., & Ingvar, M. (2001). Reactivation of motor brain areas during explicit memory for actions. NeuroImage, 20, 1–8.Google Scholar
  45. Olofsson, U. (1997). Win some, lose some: Hypermnesia for actions reflects increased item-specific processing. Memory & Cognition, 25, 797–800.Google Scholar
  46. Perea, M., & Rosa, E. (2002). The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological Research, 66, 180–194.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Roencker, D. L., Thompson, C. P., & Brown, S. C. (1971). Comparison of measures for the estimation of clustering in free recall. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 45–48.Google Scholar
  48. Seiler, K., & Engelkamp, J. (2003). The role of item-specific information for the serial position curve in free recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 954–964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Serra, M., & Nairne, J. S. (1993). Design controversies and the generation effect: Support for an item-order hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 21, 34–40.Google Scholar
  50. Smith, R. E., & Hunt, R. R. (2000). The effects of distinctiveness require reinstatement of organization: The importance of intentional memory instructions. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 431–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Sternberg, R. J., & Tulving, E. (1977). The measurement of subjective organization in free recall. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 539–556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wippich, W. (1980). Bildhaftigkeit und Organisation: Untersuchungen zu einer differenzierten Organisationshypothese. Darmstadt, Germany: Steinkopff.Google Scholar
  53. Zimmer, H. D. (2001). Why do actions speak louder than words: Action memory as a variant of encoding manipulations or the result of a specific memory system? In H. D. Zimmer, et al. (Eds.), Memory for action: A distinct form of episodic memory? (pp. 151–198). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Zimmer, H. D., & Engelkamp, J. (1989). Does motor encoding enhance relational information? Psychological Research, 51, 158–167.Google Scholar
  55. Zimmer, H. D., & Engelkamp, J. (1999). Levels of processing effects in subject-performed tasks. Memory & Cognition, 27, 907–914.Google Scholar
  56. Zimmer, H. D., & Mohr, M. (1986). Organisation und Organisierbarkeit von Verben und Substantiven bei einer verbal-semantischen bzw. ‘modalitätsspezifischen’ Lernweise (Arbeiten der Fachrichtung Psychologie Nr. 100). [Organisation and facilities of organisation of verbs and nouns after semantic or modality-specific encoding.] Saarbrücken, Germany: Universität des Saarlandes.Google Scholar
  57. Zimmer, H. D., Helstrup, T., & Engelkamp, J. (2000). Pop-out into memory: A retrieval mechanism that is enhanced with the recall of subject-performed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26, 658–670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2003

Authors and Affiliations

  • Johannes Engelkamp
    • 1
  • Kerstin H. Seiler
    • 1
  • Hubert D. Zimmer
    • 1
  1. 1.FR PsychologieSaarland UniversitySaarbrückenGermany

Personalised recommendations