Biological Cybernetics

, 100:185 | Cite as

A neurally plausible model of the dynamics of motion integration in smooth eye pursuit based on recursive Bayesian estimation

Original Paper

Abstract

In this study, we describe a model of motion integration in smooth eye pursuit based on a recursive Bayesian estimation process, which displays a dynamic behaviour qualitatively similar to the dynamics of the motion integration process observed experimentally, both psychophysically in humans and monkeys, and physiologically in monkeys. By formulating the model as an approximate version of a Kalman filter algorithm, we have been able to show that it can be put into a neurally plausible, distributed recurrent form which coarsely corresponds to the recurrent circuitry of visual cortical areas V1 and MT. The model thus provides further support for the notion that the motion integration process is based on a form of Bayesian estimation, as has been suggested by many psychophysical studies, and moreover suggests that the observed dynamic properties of this process are the result of the recursive nature of the motion estimation.

Keywords

Visual perception Motion integration Smooth eye pursuit Bayesian estimation 

References

  1. Adelson EH, Movshon JA (1982) Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns. Nature 300: 523–525CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Barthelemy FV, Perrinet LU, Castet E, Masson GS (2008) Dynamics of distributed 1D and 2D motion representations for short latency ocular following. Vision Res 48: 501–522CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bayerl P, Neumann H (2004) Disambiguating visual motion through contextual feedback modulation. Neural Comp 16: 2041–2066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Born RT, Groh JM, Zhao R, Lukasewycz SJ (2000) Segregation of object and background motion in visual area MT: effects of microstimulation on eye movements. Neuron 26: 725–734CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Born RT, Pack CC, Ponce CR, Si Yi (2006) Temporal evolution of 2-dimensional direction signals used to guide eye movements. J Neurophysiol 95: 284–300PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Born RT, Pack CC, Zhao R (2002) Integration of motion cues for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements. Prog Brain Res 140: 225–237CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bowns L (1996) Evidence for a feature tracking explanation of why type II plaids move in the vector sum directions at short durations. Vision Res 36: 3685–3694CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Burke D, Wenderoth P (1993) The effect of interactions between one dimensional component gratings on two dimensional motion perception. Vision Res 33: 343–350CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Movshon JA (1997) Linearity and normalization in simple cells of the macaque primary visual cortex. J Neurosci 17: 8621–8644PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Donnelly M, Bowd C, Patterson R (1997) Direction discrimination of cyclopean (stereoscopic) and luminance motion. Vision Res 37(15): 2041–2046CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Fennema CL, Thompson W (1979) Velocity determination in scenes containing several moving objects. Comput Graph Image Process 9: 301–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Groh JH, Born RT, Newsome WT (1997) How is a sensory map read out? Effects of microstimulation in visual area MT on saccades and smooth pursuit eye movements. J Neurosci 17: 4312–4330PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Grossberg S, Mingolla E, Viswanathan L (2001) Neural dynamics of motion integration and segmentation within and across apertures. Vision Res 41: 2521–2553CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Heeger DJ, Simoncelli EP (1993) Model of visual motion sensing. In: Harris L, Jenkin M (eds) Spatial vision in humans and robots. Cambridge University Press, pp 367–92Google Scholar
  15. Hupé JM, James AC, Girard P, Lomber SG, Payne BR, Bullier J (2001) Feedback connections act on the early part of the responses in monkey visual cortex. J Neurophysiol 85: 134–145PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Kalman RE (1960) A new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. Trans ASME J Basic Eng 82: 35–45Google Scholar
  17. Koechlin E, Anton JL, Burnod Y (1999) Bayesian inference in populations of cortical neurons: a model of motion integration and segmentation in area MT. Biol Cybern 80: 25–44CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Komatsu H, Wurtz RH (1988) Relation of cortical areas MT and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and visual properties of neurons. J Neurophysiol 60: 580–603PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Komatsu H, Wurtz RH (1989) Modulation of pursuit eye movements by stimulation of cortical areas MT and MST. J Neurophysiol 62: 31–47PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Lidén L, Pack C (1999) The role of terminators and occlusion cues in motion integration and segmentation: a neural network model. Vision Res 39: 3301–3320CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Linder A, Ilg UW (2000) Initiation of smooth-pursuit eye movements to first-order and second-order motion stimuli. Exp Brain Res 133: 450–456CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lisberger SG, Westbrook LE (1985) Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys. J Neurosci 5: 1662–1673PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Livingstone MS, Pack CC, Born RT (2001) Two-dimensional substructure of MT receptive fields. Neuron 30: 781–793CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Löffler G, Orbach HS (1999) Computing feature motion without feature detectors: a model for terminator motion without end-stopped cells. Vision Res 39: 859–871CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Lorenceau J, Shiffrar M, Wells N, Castet E (1993) Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines. Vision Res 33: 1207–1217CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Marr D, Ullman S (1981) Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 211: 151–180CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Masson GS, Castet E (2002) Parallel motion processing for the initiation of short-latency ocular following in humans. J Neurosci 22: 5149–5163PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Masson GS, Rybarczyk Y, Castet E, Mestre DR (2000) Temporal dynamics of motion integration for the initiation of tracking eye movements at ultra-short latencies. Vis Neurosci 17: 753–767CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Masson GS, Stone LS (2002) From following edges to pursuing objects. J Neurophysiol 88: 2869–2873CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Maunsell JH, Van Essen DC (1983) The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 3: 2563–2586PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Montagnini A, Mamassian P, Perrinet L, Castet E, Masson GS (2007) Bayesian modeling of dynamic motion integration. J Physiol Paris 101: 64–77CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Movshon JA, Adelson EH, Gizzi MS, Newsome WT (1985) The analysis of moving visual patterns. Pattern recognition mechanisms. In: Chagas C, Gattass R, Gross C (eds) Pontificiae Academiae Scientiarum Scripta Varia, vol 54, pp 117–151. Vatican Press, Rome (Reprinted in Exp Brain Res, Supplementum 11: 117–151, 1986)Google Scholar
  33. Pack CC, Born RT (2001) Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain. Nature 409: 1040–1042CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Pack CC, Gartland AJ, Born RT (2004) Integration of contour and terminator signals in visual area MT. J Neurosci 24: 3268–3280CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Pack CC, Livingstone MS, Duffy KR, Born RT (2003) End-stopping and the aperture problem: two-dimensional motion signals in macaque V1. Neuron 39: 671–680CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Patterson R, Bowd C, Donnelly M (1998) The cyclopean (stereoscopic) barber pole illusion. Vision Res 38: 2119–2125CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Polat U, Mizobe K, Pettet MW, Kasamatsu T, Norcia AM (1998) Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell’s contrast threshold. Nature 391: 580–584CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Raiguel SE, Lagae L, Gulya SB, Orban GA (1989) Response latencies of visual cells in macaque areas V1, V2 and V5. Brain Res 493: 155–159CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Rao RPN (2004) Bayesian computation in recurrent neural circuits. Neural Comp 16: 1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Rockland A, Knutson T (2000) Feedback connections from area MT of the squirrel monkey to areas V1 and V2. J Comp Neurol 425: 345–368CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Rust NC, Mante V, Simoncelli EP, Movshon JA (2006) How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns. Nat Neurosci 9: 1421–1431CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Sceniak MP, Ringach DL, Hawken MJ, Shapley R (1999) Contrast’s effect on spatial summation by macaque V1 neurons. Nat Neurosci 2: 733–739CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Sceniak MP, Hawken MJ, Shapley R (2001) Visual spatial characterization of macaque V1 neurons. J Neurophysiol 85: 1873–1887PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Schmolesky MT, Youngchang Wang, Hanes DP, Thompson KG, Leutgeb S, Schall JD (1998) Signal timing across the macaque visual system. J Neurophysiol 79: 3272–3278PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Shibata T, Tabata H, Schaal S, Kawato M (2005) A model of smooth pursuit based on learning of the target dynamics using only retinal signals. Neural Netw 18: 213–225CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Shipp S, Zeki S (1989) The organization of connections between areas V5 and V1 in macaque monkey visual cortex. Eur J Neurosci 1: 309–332CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Simoncelli EP (2003) Local analysis of visual motion. In: Chalupa LM, Werner JS (eds) The visual neurosciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 1616–1623Google Scholar
  48. Simoncelli E, Adelson E, Heeger D (1991) Probability distributions of optical flow. In: Proceedings of IEEE Conference on Computer Vision Pattern Recoginition, IEEE, Washington DC, pp 310–15Google Scholar
  49. Simoncelli EP, Heeger DJ (1998) A model of neuronal responses in visual area MT. Vision Res 38: 743–761CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Smith MA, Majaj NJ, Movshon JA (2005) Dynamics of motion signaling by neurons in macaque area MT. Nature Neurosci 8: 220–228CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Stocker AA, Simoncelli EP (2006) Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception. Nat Neurosci 9: 578–585CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Stone L, Thompson P (1990) Human speed perception is contrast dependent. Vision Res 32: 1535–1549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Stone L, Watson A, Mulligan J (1990) Effect of contrast on the perceived direction of a moving plaid. Vision Res 30: 1049–1067CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Van Essen D, Maunsell J, Bixby J (1981) The middle temporal visual area in macaque: myeloarchitecture, connections, functional properties and topographic organization. J Comp Neurol 199: 292–326CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Wallace JM, Stone LS, Masson GS (2005) Object motion computation for the initiation of smooth pursuit eye movements in humans. J Neurophysiol 93: 2279–2293CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Wallach H (1935) Über visuell wahrgenommene Bewegungsrichtung. Psychol Forsch 20: 325–380CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Weiss E, Simoncelli EP, Adelson EH (2002) Motion illusions as optimal percepts. Nat Neurosci 5: 598–604CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Wilson HR, Ferrera VP, Yo C (1992) A psychophysically motivated model for two-dimensional motion perception. Visual Neurosci 9: 79–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Wuerger S, Shapley R, Rubin N (1996) On the visually perceived direction of motion by Hans Wallach: 60 years later. Perception 25: 1317–1367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Yo C, Wilson H (1992) Moving two-dimensional patterns can capture the perceived directions of lower or higher spatial frequency gratings. Vision Res 32: 1263–1270CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Computational and Theoretical NeuroscienceUniversity of PlymouthPlymouthUK

Personalised recommendations