Effects of dawn simulation on markers of sleep inertia and post-waking performance in humans
- 696 Downloads
- 14 Citations
Abstract
Purpose
To examine the effects of a simulated dawn during the last 30 min of sleep on the subsequent dissipation of sleep inertia and changes in simulated work and physical performance.
Methods
Eight participants, who reported difficulty with morning waking, were administered in a random order to a control (C) and a dawn simulation (DS) trial (starting 30 min prior to waking). Subjective ratings of sleep quality and alertness were obtained alongside measures of cognitive performance (addition and reaction time tasks measured at 5, 30 and 75 min after waking at habitual workday times). Physical performance was also measured 35 min after waking using a self-paced cycling protocol.
Results
After waking in DS, perceived sleep quality was 1.16 ± 0.89 (p = 0.01) points higher compared with C. Ratings of alertness were significantly higher in DS than C throughout the testing period (p = 0.04). Cognitive performance improved in both trials as time awake increased (p < 0.0005). On average, participants completed a greater number of additions in DS compared with C (69.5 ± 15.3 vs 66.9 ± 16.7, p = 0.03). Reaction times were also faster in DS compared with C (0.81 ± 0.07 s vs 0.86 ± 0.06 s, p < 0.0005). The self-paced time-trial was completed 21.4 s (4.7 %) quicker in DS (p = 0.07).
Conclusion
These data provide the first evidence that light exposure during the last 30 min of habitual sleep can increase subjective alertness and improve both cognitive and physical performance after waking.
Keywords
Artificial dawn Sleep inertia Performance PhototherapyAbbreviations
- 95 % CI
95 % Confidence interval
- C
Control condition
- DS
Dawn simulation condition
- ELISA
Enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay
- KSS
Karolinska sleepiness scale
- RPE
Rating of perceived exertion
- s
Second
- SD
Standard deviation
- Tc
Intestinal temperature
- VAS
Visual analogue scale
Notes
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Lumie for providing financial support for this study. The authors have no financial interest associated with the outcome of this study and declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- Åkerstedt T, Gillberg M (1990) Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 52:29–37PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Altareki N, Drust B, Atkinson G, Cable T, Gregson W (2009) Effects of environmental heat stress (35°C) with simulated air movement on the thermoregulatory responses during a 4-km cycling time trial. Int J Sports Med 30:9–15PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ando K, Kripke DF (1996) Light attenuation by the human eyelid. Biol Psychiatry 39:22–25PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ballinger GA (2004) Using generalized estimating equations for longitudinal data analysis. Organ Res Methods 7:127–150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Benloucif S, Burgess HJ, Klerman EB, Lewy AJ, Middleton B, Murphy PJ, Parry BL, Revell VL (2008) Measuring melatonin in humans. J Clin Sleep Med 4:66–69PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bergh U, Ekblom B (1979) Influence of muscle temperature on maximal muscle strength and power output in human skeletal muscles. Acta Physiol Scand 107:33–37PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bierman A, Figueiro MG, Rea MS (2011) Measuring and predicting eyelid spectral transmittance. J Biomed Opt 16:1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bishop D (2003) Warm up I: potential mechanisms and the effects of passive warm up on exercise performance. Sports Med 33:439–454PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bland JM, Altman DG (1995) Calculating correlation coefficients with repeated observations: part 1–correlation within subjects. BMJ 310:446PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Borg GAV (1982) Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc 14:377–381PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brooks A, Lack L (2006) A brief afternoon nap following nocturnal sleep restriction: which nap duration is most recuperative? Sleep 29:831–840PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Cajochen C, Krauchi K, Wirz-Justice A (2003) Role of melatonin in the regulation of human circadian rhythms and sleep. J Neuroendocrinol 15:432–437PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Danilenko KV, Wirz-Justice A, Kräuchi K, Cajochen C, Weber JM, Fairhurst S, Terman M (2000) Phase advance after one or three simulated dawns in humans. Chronobiol Int 17:659–668PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Figueiro MG, Rea MS (2012) Preliminary evidence that light through the eyelids can suppress melatonin and phase shift dim light melatonin onset. BMC Res Notes 5:221–229PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gabel V, Maire M, Reichert CF, Chellappa SL, Schmidt C, Hommes V, Viola AU, Cajochen C (2013) Effects of artificial dawn and morning blue light on daytime cognitive performance, well-being, cortisol and melatonin levels. Chronobiol Int 30:988–997PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Giménez MC, Hessels M, van de Werken M, de Vries B, Beersma DGM, Gordijn MCM (2010) Effects of artificial dawn on subjective ratings of sleep inertia and dim light melatonin onset. Chronobiol Int 27:1219–1241PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hancock PA, Vazmatzidis I (2003) Effects of heat stress on cognitive performance: the current state of knowledge. Int J Hyperth 19:355–372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Horne JA, Ostberg O (1976) A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness–eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4:97–110PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Jewett ME, Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Khalsa SB, Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA (1999) Time course of sleep inertia dissipation in human performance and alertness. J Sleep Res 8:1–8PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kleitman N (1964) The evolutionary theory of sleep and wakefulness. Perspect Biol Med 7:169–178PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lewy AJ, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK, Newsome DA, Markey SP (1980) Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science 210:1267–1269PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Masino SA, Dunwiddie TV (1999) Temperature-dependent modulation of excitatory transmission in hippocampal slices is mediated by extracellular adenosine. J Neurosci 19:1932–1939PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Masino S, Dunwiddie T (2000) A transient increase in temperature induces persistent potentiation of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal slices. Neuroscience 101:907–912PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Norden MJ, Avery DH (1993) A controlled study of dawn simulation in subsyndromal winter depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 88:67–71PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ramanathan NL (1964) A new weighting system for mean surface temperature of the human body. J Appl Physiol 19:531–533PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Reilly T, Edwards B (2007) Altered sleep–wake cycles and physical performance in athletes. Physiol Behav 90:274–284PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Roenneberg T, Wirz-Justice A, Merrow M (2003) Life between clocks: daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. J Biol Rhythms 18:80–90PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Taillard J, Philip P, Coste O, Sagaspe P, Bioulac B (2003) The circadian and homeostatic modulation of sleep pressure during wakefulness differs between morning and evening chronotypes. J Sleep Res 12:275–282PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tassi P, Muzet A (2000) Sleep inertia. Sleep Med Rev 4:341–353PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Terman M, Schlager D, Fairhurst S, Perlman B (1989) Dawn and dusk simulation as a therapeutic intervention. Biol Psychiatry 25:966–970PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Thorn L, Hucklebridge F (2004) The effect of dawn simulation on the cortisol response to awakening in healthy participants. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29:925–930PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Van De Werken M, Giménez MC, Vries B, Beersma DGM, Van Someren EUSJW, Gordijn M (2010) Effects of artificial dawn on sleep inertia, skin temperature, and the awakening cortisol response. J Sleep Res 19:425–435CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Van Marken Lichtenbelt WD, Daanen HAM, Wouters L, Fronczek R, Raymann RJEM, Severens NMW, Van Someren EJW (2006) Evaluation of wireless determination of skin temperature using iButtons. Physiol Behav 88:489–497PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wertz AT, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP Jr (2006) Effects of sleep inertia on cognition. JAMA 295:163–164PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wilkinson R, Fox R, Goldsmith R, Hampton I, Lewis H (1964) Psychological and physiological responses to raised body temperature. J Appl Physiol 19:287–291PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23:497–509PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wright KP Jr, Hull JT, Czeisler CA (2002) Relationship between alertness, performance, and body temperature in humans. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 283:1370–1377Google Scholar
- Zavada A, Gordijn MCM, Beersma DGM, Daan S, Roenneberg T (2005) Comparison of the Munich Chronotype questionnaire with the Horne-Östberg’s morningness–eveningness score. Chronobiol Int 22:267–278PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar