Journal of Comparative Physiology A

, Volume 196, Issue 11, pp 853–867 | Cite as

Active wall following by Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus)

Original Paper

Abstract

When introduced into a novel environment that limits or prevents vision, a variety of species including Mexican blind cavefish (Astyanax mexicanus) exhibit wall-following behaviors. It is often assumed that wall following serves an exploratory function, but this assertion remains untested against alternative artifactual explanations. Here, we test whether wall following by cavefish is a purposeful behavior in which fish actively maintain a close relationship with the wall, or an artifactual consequence of being enclosed in a small concave arena, in which fish turn slightly to avoid the wall whenever it impedes forward movement. Wall-following abilities of fish were tested in a large, goggle-shaped arena, where forward motion along the convex wall was unimpeded. In this circumstance, cavefish continued to follow the wall at frequencies significantly above chance levels. Lateral line inactivation significantly reduced the ability of fish to follow convex, but not concave or straight, walls. Wall-following abilities of normal fish decreased with decreasing radius of wall convex curvature. Our results demonstrate that cavefish actively follow walls of varying contours. Radius-of-curvature effects coupled with the difficulties posed by convex walls to lateral line-deprived fish suggest a partially complementary use of tactile and lateral line information to regulate distance from the wall.

Keywords

Astyanax Wall following Exploratory behavior Lateral line Spatial cognition 

Abbreviations

IR

Infrared

BGSU

Bowling Green State University

LED

Light emitting diode

CoCl2

Cobalt chloride

References

  1. Abdel-Latif H, Hassan E-S, Campenhausen C (1990) Sensory performance of blind Mexican cave fish after destruction of the canal neuromasts. Naturwissenschaften 77:237–239CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker CF, Montgomery JC (1999) The sensory basis of rheotaxis in the blind Mexican cavefish, Asytanax fasciatus. J Comp Physiol A 184:519–527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barnett SA (1963) The rat: a study in behavior. Methuen, LondonGoogle Scholar
  4. Basel J, Sandemann D (2000) Crayfish (Cherax destructor) use tactile cues to detect and learn topographic changes in their environment. Ethology 106:247–259CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Besson M, Martin J-R (2004) Centrophobism/thigmotaxis, a new role for the mushroom bodies in Drosophila. J Neurobiol 62:386–396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Breder CM (1942) Descriptive ecology of La Cueva Chica, with especial reference to the blind fish, Anoptichthys. Zool Sci Contrib N Y Zool Soc 27:7–15Google Scholar
  7. Breder CM (1943) Chemical sensory reaction in the Mexican blind characins. Zool Sci Contrib N Y Zool Soc 28:169–200Google Scholar
  8. Burt de Perera T (2004) Fish can encode order in their spatial map. Proc R Soc Biol Sci Ser B 271:2131–2134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Creed RP, Miller JR (1990) Interpreting animal wall-following behavior. Experientia 46:758–761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Dijkgraaf S (1963) The functioning and significance of the lateral-line organs. Biol Rev 38:51–105CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Gertychowa R (1970) Studies on the ethology and space orientation of the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani Hubbs et Innes 1936 (Characidae). Folia Biol 18:9–69Google Scholar
  12. Hassan E-S (1985) Mathematical analysis of the stimulus for the lateral line organ. Biol Cybern 52:23–36CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Hassan E-S (1989) Hydrodynamic imaging of the surroundings by the lateral line of the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani. In: Coombs S, Görner P, Münz H (eds) The mechanosensory lateral line: neurobiology and evolution. Springer, New York, pp 217–227Google Scholar
  14. Hassan E-S (1992a) Mathematical description of the stimuli to the lateral line system of fish derived from a three-dimensional flow field analysis. II. The case of gliding alongside or above a plane surface. Biol Cybern 66:453–461CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hassan E-S (1992b) Mathematical description of the stimuli to the lateral line system of fish derived from a three-dimensional flow field analysis I. The case of moving in open water and of gliding towards a plane surface. Biol Cybern 66:443–452CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hassan E-S (1993a) Mathematical description of the stimuli to the lateral line system of fish derived from the three-dimensional flow field analysis. Biol Cybern 69:525–538Google Scholar
  17. Hassan E-S (1993b) Mathematical description of the stimuli to the lateral line system of fish, derived from a 3-dimensional flow field analysis III. The case of an oscillating sphere near the fish. Biol Cybern 69:525–538Google Scholar
  18. Hassan E-S, Abdel-Latif R, Biebricher R (1992) Studies on the effects of Ca++ and Co++ on the swimming behavior of the blind Mexican cave fish. J Comp Physiol A 171:413–419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hill EW, Rieser JJ, Hill MM, Hill M, Halpin J, Halpin R (1993) How persons with visual impairments explore novel spaces: strategies of good and poor performers. J Vis Impair Blind 87:295–301Google Scholar
  20. Janssen J (2000) Toxicity of CoCl2: implications for lateral line studies. J Comp Physiol A 186:957–960CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Jeanson R, Blanco S, Fournier R, Deneubourg J-L, Fourcassie V, Theraulaz G (2003) A model of animal movements in a bounded space. J Theor Biol 225:443–451CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Jeffery WR (2001) Cavefish as a model system in evolutionary developmental biology. Dev Biol 231:1–12CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. John KR (1957) Observations on the behavior of blind and blinded fishes. Copeia 1957:123–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kallai J, Makany T, Karadi K, Jacobs WJ (2004) Spatial orientation strategies in Morris-type virtual water task for humans. Behav Brain Res 159:187–196CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Kallai J, Makany T, Nadel L, Jacobs JW, Csatho A, Karadi K, Horvath D, Kovacs-Labadi B, Jarai R (2007) Cognitive and affective aspects of thigmotaxis strategy in humans. Behav Neurosci 121:21–30CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  26. Karlsen HE, Sand O (1987) Selective and reversible blocking of the lateral line in freshwater fish. J Exp Biol 133:249–262Google Scholar
  27. Martin J-R (2004) A portrait of locomotor behavior in Drosophila determined by a video-tracking paradigm. Behav Process 67:207–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Meyers JR, Macdonald RB, Duggan A, Lenzi D, Standaert DG, Corwin JT, Corey DP (2003) Lighting up the senses: FM1-43 loading of sensory cells through nonselective ion channels. J Neurosci 23:4054–4065PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Mitchell HC, Russell WH, Elliott WR (1977) Mexican eyeless characin fishes, genus Astyanax: environment, distribution and evolution. Texas Tech Press, LubbockGoogle Scholar
  30. Montgomery JC, Coombs S, Baker CF (2001) The mechanosensory lateral line system of the hypogean form of Astyanax fasciatus. Environ Biol Fishes 62:87–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Paglianti A, Messena G, Cianfanelli A, Berti R (2006) Is perception of their own odour effective in orienting the exploratory activity of cave fishes. Can J Zool 84:871–876CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Repass JJ, Watson GM (2001) Anemone repair proteins as a potential therapeutic agent for vertebrate hair cells: facilitated recovery of the lateral line of blind cave fish. Hear Res 154:98–107CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Sand O (1975) Effects of different ionic environments on the mechanosensitivity of lateral line organs in the mudpuppy. J Comp Physiol A 102:27–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schemmel C (1974) Genetische Untersuchungen zur Evolution des Geschmacksinnesapparates bei cavernicolen Fischen. J Zool Syst Evol Res 12:196–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Schemmel C (1980) Studies in the genetics of feeding behavior in the cave fish Astyanax mexicanus f. Anoptichthys. An example of apparent monofactorial inheritance by polygenes. Z Tierpsychol 53:9–22CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Sharma S, Coombs S, Patton P, Burt de Perera T (2009) The function of wall-following behaviors in the Mexican blind cavefish and a sighted relative, the Mexican tetra (Astyanax). J Comp Physiol A 195:225–240CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Simon P, Dupuis R, Costentin J (1994) Thigmotaxis as an index of anxiety in mice: influence of dopaminergic transmissions. Behav Brain Res 61:59–64CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Teyke T (1985) Collision with and avoidance of obstacles by blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani (Characidae). J Comp Physiol A 157:837–843CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Teyke T (1988) Flow field, swimming velocity and boundary layer: parameters which affect the stimulus for the lateral line organ in blind fish. J Comp Physiol A 163:53–61CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Teyke T (1989) Learning and remembering the environment in the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani. J Comp Physiol A 164:655–662CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Teyke T (1990) Morphological differences in neuromasts of the blind cave fish Astyanax hubbsi and the sighted river fish Astyanax mexicanus. Brain Behav Evol 35:23–30CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Treit D, Fundytus M (1988) Thigmotaxis as a test of anxiolytic activity in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 31:959–962CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. van Trump W, Coombs S, Duncan K, McHenry MJ (2010) Gentamicin is ototoxic to all hair cells in the fish lateral line system. Hear Res 261:42–50CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. von Campenhausen C, Riess I, Weissert R (1981) Detection of stationary objects by the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani (Characidae). J Comp Physiol A 143:369–374CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Voneida TJ, Fish SE (1984) Central nervous system changes related to the reduction of visual input in a naturally blind fish (Astyanax hubbsi). Am Zool 24:775–782Google Scholar
  46. Weissert R, von Campenhausen C (1981) Discrimination between stationary objects by the blind cave fish Anoptichthys jordani. J Comp Physiol A 143:375–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Windsor S, Tan D, Montgomery JC (2008) Swimming kinematics and hydrodynamic imaging in the blind Mexican cave fish (Astyanax fasciatus). J Exp Biol 211:2950–2959CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Biological Sciences and JP Scott Center for Neuroscience, Mind and BehaviorBowling Green State UniversityBowling GreenUSA
  2. 2.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of AucklandAucklandNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations