Journal of Comparative Physiology A

, Volume 195, Issue 6, pp 547–555 | Cite as

Cuttlefish use visual cues to control three-dimensional skin papillae for camouflage

  • Justine J. Allen
  • Lydia M. Mäthger
  • Alexandra Barbosa
  • Roger T. Hanlon
Original Paper

Abstract

Cephalopods (octopus, squid and cuttlefish) are known for their camouflage. Cuttlefish Sepia officinalis use chromatophores and light reflectors for color change, and papillae to change three-dimensional physical skin texture. Papillae vary in size, shape and coloration; nine distinct sets of papillae are described here. The objective was to determine whether cuttlefish use visual or tactile cues to control papillae expression. Cuttlefish were placed on natural substrates to evoke the three major camouflage body patterns: Uniform/Stipple, Mottle and Disruptive. Three versions of each substrate were presented: the actual substrate, the actual substrate covered with glass (removes tactile information) and a laminated photograph of the substrate (removes tactile and three-dimensional information because depth-of-field information is unavailable). No differences in Small dorsal papillae or Major lateral mantle papillae expression were observed among the three versions of each substrate. Thus, visual (not tactile) cues drive the expression of papillae in S. officinalis. Two sets of papillae (Major lateral mantle papillae and Major lateral eye papillae) showed irregular responses; their control requires future investigation. Finally, more Small dorsal papillae were shown in Uniform/Stipple and Mottle patterns than in Disruptive patterns, which may provide clues regarding the visual mechanisms of background matching versus disruptive coloration.

Keywords

Cephalopod behavior Sepia officinalis Visual perception Texture Dynamic camouflage 

References

  1. Aristotle (1910) Historia Animalium. Clarendon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  2. Barbosa A, Florio CF, Chiao C-C, Hanlon RT (2004) Visual background features that elicit mottled body patterns in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Biol Bull 207:154Google Scholar
  3. Barbosa A, Mäthger LM, Chubb C, Florio C, Chiao C-C, Hanlon RT (2007) Disruptive coloration in cuttlefish: a visual perception mechanism that regulates ontogenetic adjustment of skin patterning. J Exp Biol 210:1139–1147PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barbosa A, Mäthger LM, Buresch KC, Kelly J, Chubb C, Chiao C-C, Hanlon RT (2008) Cuttlefish camouflage: the effects of substrate contrast and size in evoking uniform, mottle or disruptive body patterns. Vis Res 48:1242–1253PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bilecenoglu M, Taşkavak E, Kunt KB (2002) Range extension of three lessepsian migrant fish (Fistularia commersoni, Sphyraena flavicauda, Lagocephalus suezensis) in the Mediterranean Sea. J Mar Biol Assoc U K 82:525–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boal JG, Ni JN (1996) Ventilation rate of cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis, in response to visual stimuli. Veliger 39:342–347Google Scholar
  7. Burr DC, Morrone MC, Spinelli D (1989) Evidence for edge and bar detectors in human vision. Vis Res 29:419–431PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chiao C-C, Hanlon RT (2001a) Cuttlefish camouflage: visual perception of size, contrast and number of white squares on artificial substrata initiates disruptive coloration. J Exp Biol 204:2119–2125PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Chiao C-C, Hanlon RT (2001b) Cuttlefish cue visually on area—not shape or aspect ratio—of light objects in the substrate to produce disruptive body patterns for camouflage. Biol Bull 201:269–270PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chiao C-C, Kelman EJ, Hanlon RT (2005) Disruptive body patterning of cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) requires visual information regarding edges and contrast of objects in natural substrate backgrounds. Biol Bull 208:7–11PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Coll J, Linde M, García-Rubies A, Riera F, Grau AM (2004) Spear fishing in the Balearic Islands (west central Mediterranean): species affected and catch evolution during the period 1975–2001. Fish Res 70:97–111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cott HB (1940) Adaptive coloration in animals. Methuen, LondonGoogle Scholar
  13. Cuthill IC, Stevens M, Sheppard J, Maddocks T, Párraga CA, Trosclanko TS (2005) Disruptive coloration and background pattern matching. Nature 434:72–74PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. de Sylva DP (1963) Systematics and life history of the great barracuda Sphyraena barracuda (Walbaum). In: Studies in tropical oceanography, vol 1. Institute of Marine Science, University of Miami Press, MiamiGoogle Scholar
  15. de Sylva DP (1990) Sphyraenidae. CLOFETA 2:860–864Google Scholar
  16. Hanlon RT (2007) Cephalopod dynamic camouflage. Curr Biol 17:R400–R405PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hanlon RT, Messenger JB (1988) Adaptive coloration in young cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L.): the morphology and development of body patterns and their relation to behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 320:437–487CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hanlon RT, Messenger JB (1996) Cephalopod behaviour. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  19. Hanlon RT, Chiao C-C, Mäthger LM, Barbosa A, Buresch KC, Chubb C (2009) Cephalopod dynamic camouflage: bridging the continuum between background matching and disruptive coloration. Philos Trans R Soc B 364:429–437CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Holmes W (1940) The colour changes and colour patterns of Sepia officinalis L. Proc Zool Soc Lond A 110:2–35Google Scholar
  21. Kelman EJ, Baddeley RJ, Shohet AJ, Osorio D (2007) Perception of visual texture and the expression of disruptive camouflage by the cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 274:1369–1375CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kelman EJ, Osorio D, Baddeley RJ (2008) A review of cuttlefish camouflage and object recognition and evidence for depth perception. J Exp Biol 211:1757–1763PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Kier WM, Messenger JB, Miyan JA (1985) Mechanoreceptors in the fins of the cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. J Exp Biol 119:369–373Google Scholar
  24. King AJ, Adamo SA (2006) The ventilatory, cardiac and behavioral responses of resting cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis L.) to sudden visual stimuli. J Exp Biol 209:1101–1111PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Langridge KV (2006) Symmetrical crypsis and asymmetrical signalling in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 273:959–967CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Marshall NJ, Messenger JB (1996) Colour-blind camouflage. Nature 382:408–409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mäthger LM, Barbosa A, Miner S, Hanlon RT (2006) Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay. Vis Res 46:1746–1753PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mäthger LM, Chiao C-C, Barbosa A, Buresch KC, Kaye S, Hanlon RT (2007) Disruptive coloration elicited on controlled natural substrates in cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. J Exp Biol 210:2657–2666PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Messenger JB (2001) Cephalopod chromatophores: neurobiology and natural history. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 76:473–528PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Osorio D, Srinivasan MV (1991) Camouflage by edge enhancement in animal coloration patterns and its implications for visual mechanisms. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 244:81–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Petrakis G, Stergiou KI (1995) Weight-length relationships for 33 fish species in Greek waters. Fish Res 21:465–469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Porter HT, Motta PJ (2004) A comparison of strike and prey capture kinematics of three species of piscivorous fishes: Florida gar (Lepisosteus platyrhincus), redfin needlefish (Strongylura notata) and great barracuda (Sphyraena barracuda). Mar Biol 145:989–1000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Randall JE, Randall HA (1960) Examples of mimicry and protective resemblance in tropical marine fishes. Bull Mar Sci Gulf Caribb 10:444–480Google Scholar
  34. Shohet AJ, Baddeley RJ, Anderson JC, Kelman EJ, Osorio D (2006) Cuttlefish responses to visual orientation of substrates, water flow and a model of motion camouflage. J Exp Biol 209:4717–4723PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Shohet AJ, Baddeley RJ, Anderson JC, Osorio D (2007) Cuttlefish camouflage: a quantitative study of patterning. Biol J Linn Soc Lond 92:335–345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Stevens M, Cuthill IC, Windsor AMM, Walker HJ (2006) Disruptive contrast in animal camouflage. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 273:2433–2438CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Thayer GH (1909) Concealing-coloration in the animal kingdom. An exposition of the laws of disguise through color and pattern: being a summary of Abbott H. Thayer’s discoveries. The Macmillan Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Justine J. Allen
    • 1
  • Lydia M. Mäthger
    • 1
  • Alexandra Barbosa
    • 1
    • 2
  • Roger T. Hanlon
    • 1
  1. 1.Marine Biological LaboratoryMarine Resources CenterWoods HoleUSA
  2. 2.Institute of Biomedical Sciences Abel Salazar (ICBAS)University of PortoPortoPortugal

Personalised recommendations