Journal of Comparative Physiology A

, Volume 193, Issue 7, pp 765–774 | Cite as

Neurophysiological response selectivity for conspecific songs over synthetic sounds in the auditory forebrain of non-singing female songbirds

  • Mark E. Hauber
  • Phillip Cassey
  • Sarah M. N. Woolley
  • Frederic E. Theunissen
Original Paper

Abstract

Female choice plays a critical role in the evolution of male acoustic displays. Yet there is limited information on the neurophysiological basis of female songbirds’ auditory recognition systems. To understand the neural mechanisms of how non-singing female songbirds perceive behaviorally relevant vocalizations, we recorded responses of single neurons to acoustic stimuli in two auditory forebrain regions, the caudal lateral mesopallium (CLM) and Field L, in anesthetized adult female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Using various metrics of response selectivity, we found consistently higher response strengths for unfamiliar conspecific songs compared to tone pips and white noise in Field L but not in CLM. We also found that neurons in the left auditory forebrain had lower response strengths to synthetics sounds, leading to overall higher neural selectivity for song in neurons of the left hemisphere. This laterality effect is consistent with previously published behavioral data in zebra finches. Overall, our results from Field L are in parallel and from CLM are in contrast with the patterns of response selectivity reported for conspecific songs over synthetic sounds in male zebra finches, suggesting some degree of sexual dimorphism of auditory perception mechanisms in songbirds.

Keywords

Auditory cortex Lateralization Mate choice Natural sounds Vocal recognition 

References

  1. Amin N, Grace GA, Theunissen FE (2004) Neural response to bird’s own song and tutor song in the zebra finch field L and caudal mesopallium. J Comp Phsysiol A 190:469–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Avey MT, Phillmore LS, MacDougall-Shackleton SA (2005) Immediate early gene expression following exposure to acoustic and visual components of courtship in zebra finches. Behav Brain Res 165:247–253PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bailey DJ, Wade J (2003) Differential expression of the immediate early genes FOS and ZENK following auditory stimulation in the juvenile male and female zebra finch. Mol Brain Res 116:147–154PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bailey DJ, Rosebush JC, Wade J (2002) The hippocampus and caudomedial neostriatum show selective responsiveness to conspecific song in the female zebra finch. J Neurobiol 52:43–51PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beecher MD, Brenowitz EA (2005) Functional aspects of song learning in birds. Trends Ecol Evol 20:143–149PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bolhuis JJ, Gahr M (2006) Neural mechanisms of birdsong memory. Nature Rev Neurosci 7:347–357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Braaten RF, Reynolds K (1999) Auditory preference for conspecific song in isolation-reared zebra finches. Anim Behav 58:105–111PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brenowitz E (1991) Altered perception of species-specific song by female birds after lesions of a forebrain nucleus. Science 251:303–305PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brenowitz EA, Beecher MD (2005) Song learning in birds: diversity and plasticity, opportunities and challenges. Trends Neurosci 28:127–132PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cardin JA, Schmidt MF (2003) Song system auditory responses are stable and highly tuned during sedation, rapidly modulated and unselective during wakefulness, and suppressed by arousal. J Neurophysiol 90:2884–2899PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Coleman MJ, Mooney R (2004) Synaptic transformations underlying highly selective auditory representations of learned birdsong. J Neurosci 24:7251–7265PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cousillas H, Richard J-P, Mathelier M, Henry L, George I, Hausberger M (2004) Experience-dependent neuronal specialization and functional organization in the central auditory area of a songbird. Eur J Neurosci 19:3343–3376PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Cousillas H, George I, Mathelier M, Richard J-P, Henry L, Hausberger M (2006) Social experience influences the development of a central auditory area. Naturwissenschaften 93:588–596PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Cynx J, Williams H, Nottebohm F (1992) Hemispheric differences in avian song discrimination. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:1372–1375PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dey DK, Ghosh SK, Mallick BK (eds) (2000) Generalized linear mixed models: a Bayesian perspective. Marcel Dekker Inc., BaselGoogle Scholar
  16. Floody OR, Arnold AP (1997) Song lateralization in the zebra finch. Horm Behav 31:25–34PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Fortune ES, Margoliash D (2004) Cytoarchitectonic organization and morphology of cells of the field L complex in male zebra finches (Taenopygia guttata). J Comp Neurol 325:388–404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gehr DD, Capsius B, Grabner P, Gahr M, Leppelsack H-J (1999) Functional organisation of the field-L-complex of adult male zebra finches. Neuroreport 10:375–380PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Gentner TZ, Margoliash D (2003) Neuronal populations and feature detectors representing learned auditory objects. Nature 424:669–674PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. George I, Cousillas H, Richard J-P, Hausberger M (2005) State-dependent hemispheric specialization in the songbird brain. J Comp Neurol 488:48–60PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. George I, Hara E, Hessler NA (2006) Behavioral and neural lateralization of vision in courtship singing of the zebra finch. J Neurobiol 66:1164–1173PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grace JA, Amin NA, Singh NC, Theunissen FE (2003) Selectivity for conspecific song in the zebra finch auditory forebrain. J Neurophysiol 89:472–487PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grim T (2006) Cuckoo growth performance in parasitized and unused hosts: not only host size matters. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 60:716–723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Grim T (2007) Experimental evidence for chick discrimination without recognition in a brood parasite host. Proc R Soc Lond B 274:373–381CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hauber ME, Sherman PW (2001) Self-referent phenotype matching: theoretical considerations and empirical evidence. Trends Neurosci 24:609–616PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hauber ME, Woolley SMN, Fremouw TE, Theunissen FE (2003) Response selectivity and tuning of auditory forebrain neurons in female zebra finches. Society for Neuroscience 34th Annual Meeting abstract, San DiegoGoogle Scholar
  27. Hauber ME, Woolley SMN, Theunissen FE (2006) Learning, memory and mate choice: early experience and neuronal discrimination of songs by female zebra finches. J Ornithol 147:S12Google Scholar
  28. Hauser MD, Andersson K (1994) Left hemisphere dominance for processing vocalizations in adult, but not infant, rhesus monkeys: field experiments. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:3946–3948PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hsu A, Woolley SMN, Fremouw T, Theunissen FE (2004) Modulation and phase spectrum of natural sounds enhance neural discrimination performed by single auditory neurons. J Neurosci 24:9201–9221PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Hunt GR, Corballis MC, Gray RD (2001) Laterality in tool manufacture by crows. Nature 414:707PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Kachman SD (2000) An introduction to generalized linear mixed models. Proceedings of a symposium at the organizational meeting for a NCR coordinating committee on “Implementation Strategies for National Beef Cattle Evaluation,” Athens, pp 59–73Google Scholar
  32. Lauay C, Gerlach NM, Adkins-Regan E, Devoogd TJ (2004) Female zebra finches require early song exposure to prefer high-quality song as adults. Anim Behav 68:1249–1255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Lauay C, Komorowski RW, Beaudin AE, DeVoogd TJ (2005) Adult female and male zebra finches show distinct patterns of spine deficits in an auditory area and in the song system when reared without exposure to normal adult song. J Comp Neurol 487:119–126PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Leppelsack HJ, Vogt M (1976) Responses of auditory neurons in the forebrain of a songbird to stimulation with species-specific sounds. J Comp Neurol 107:263–274Google Scholar
  35. Littell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD (1996) SAS system for mixed models. SAS Institute, CaryGoogle Scholar
  36. MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Hulse SH, Ball GF (1998) Neural bases of song preferences in female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Neuroreport 9:3047–3052PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Ball GF (1999) Comparative studies on sex differences in the song-control systems of songbirds. Trends Neurosci 22:432–436PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Maney DL, MacDougall-Shackleton EA, MacDougall-Shackleton SA, Ball GF, Hahn TP (2003) Immediate early gene response to hearing song correlates with receptive behavior and depends on dialect in a female songbird. J Comp Physiol A 189:667–674CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Maney DL, Cho E, Goode CT (2006) Estrogen dependent selectivity of genomic responses to birdsong. Euro J Neurosci 23:1523–1529CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Miller DB (1979) Long-term recognition of father’s song by female zebra finches. Nature 280:389–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nottebohm F, Arnold AP (1976) Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of the songbird brain. Science 194:211–213PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Palleroni A, Hauser MD (2003) Experience-dependent plasticity for auditory processing in a raptor. Science 299:1195PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Phan ML, Pytte CL, Vicario DS (2006) Early auditory experience generates long-lasting memories that may subserve vocal learning in songbirds. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:1088–1093PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Poremba A, Malloy M, Saunders RC, Carson RE, Herscovitch P, Mishkin M (2004) Species-specific calls evoke asymmetric activity in the monkey’s temporal poles. Nature 427:448–451PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pytte CL, Suthers RA (1999) A bird’s own song contributes to conspecific song perception. Neuroreport 10:1773–1778PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Reiner A, Perkel DJ, Bruce L, Butler AB, Csillag A, Kuenzel W, Medina L, Paxinos G, Shimizu T, Striedter GF, Wild M, Ball GF, Durand S, Gunturkun O, Lee DW, Mello CV, Powers A, White SA, Hough G, Kubikova L, Smulders TV, Wada K, Dugas-Ford J, Husband S, Yamamoto K, Yu J, Siang C, Jarvis ED (2004) Revised nomenclature for avian telencephalon and some related brainstem nuclei. J Comp Neurol 473:377–414PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Riebel K (2000) Early exposure to song leads to repeatable preferences for male song in female zebra finches. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:2553–2558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Riebel K (2003) The ‘mute’ sex revisited: vocal production and perception learning in female songbirds. Adv Stud Behav 33:49–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Riebel K (2003) Developmental influences on song perception in female zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata: is there a sensitive phase for song preference learning? Anim Biol 53:73–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Riebel K, Smallegange IM, Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis JJ (2002) Sexual equality in zebra finch song preference: Evidence for a dissociation between song recognition and production learning. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:729–733CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Riebel K, Hall ML, Langmore NE (2005) Female songbirds still struggling to be heard. Trends Ecol Evol 20:419–420PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Scheich H, Bonke D (1979) Responsiveness of units in the auditory neostriatum of the guineafowl (Numida meleagris). J Comp Phsyiol 132:257–276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Seddon N (2005) Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in Neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution 59:200–215PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Sockman KW, Gentner TQ, Ball GF (2002) Recent experience modulates forebrain gene-expression in response to mate-choice cues in European starlings. Proc R Soc Lond B 269:2479–2485CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis JJ, den Boer-Visser AM, ten Cate C (2005) Neuronal activation related to auditory perception in a non-songbird. J J Comp Neurol 488:342–351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Terpstra NJ, Bolhuis JJ, Riebel K, van der Burg JMM, den Boer-Visser AM (2006) Localized brain activation specific to auditory memory in a female songbird. J Comp Neurol 494:784–791PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Theunissen FE, Amin N, Shaevitz SS, Woolley SMN, Fremouw T, Hauber ME (2004) Song selectivity in the song system and in the auditory forebrain. Ann NY Acad Sci 1016:222–245PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Tomaszycki ML, Sluzas EM, Sundberg KA, Newman SW, DeVoogd TJ (2006) Immediate early gene (ZENK) responses to song in juvenile female and male zebra finches: effects of rearing environment. J Neurobiol 66:1175–1182PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Vates GE, Broome BM, Mello CV, Nottebohm F (1996) Auditory pathways of caudal telencephalon and their relation to the song system of adult male zebra finches. J Comp Neurol 366:613–642PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wild JM (1995) Convergence of somatosensory and auditory projections in the avian torus semicircularis, including the central auditory nucleus. J Comp Neurol 358:465–486PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Williams H (1990) Models for song learning in the zebra finch: fathers or others. Anim Behav 39:745–757CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Williams H, Crane LA, Hale TK, Esposito MA, Nottebohm F (1992) Right-side dominance for song control in the zebra finch. J Neurobiol 23:1006–1020PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Woolley SMN, Casseday JH (2004) Response properties of single neurons in the zebra finch auditory midbrain: response patterns, frequency coding, intensity coding and spike latencies. J Neurophysiol 91:136–151PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Woolley SMN, Casseday JH (2005) Processing of modulated sounds in the zebra finch auditory midbrain: responses to noise, frequency sweeps and sinusoidal amplitude modulations. J Neurophysiol 94:1143–1157PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Woolley SMN, Fremouw TE, Hsu A, Theunissen FE (2005) Spectro-temporal modulation tuning as a mechanism for auditory discrimination of natural sounds. Nat Neurosci 8:1371–1379PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Woolley SMN, Gill P, Theunissen FE (2006) Stimulus-dependent auditory tuning results in synchronized population coding of vocalizations in the songbird midbrain. J Neurosci 26:2499–2512PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Yamaguchi A (2001) Sex differences in vocal learning in birds. Nature 411:257–258PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Zann RA (1996) The zebra finch: a synthesis of field and laboratory studies. Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  69. Zatorre RJ (2001) Neural specializations for tonal processing. Ann NY Acad Sci 930:193–210PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer-Verlag 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mark E. Hauber
    • 1
    • 2
  • Phillip Cassey
    • 3
  • Sarah M. N. Woolley
    • 1
    • 4
  • Frederic E. Theunissen
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Psychology and Helen Wills Neuroscience InstituteUniversity of CaliforniaBerkeleyUSA
  2. 2.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of AucklandAucklandNew Zealand
  3. 3.School of Biological SciencesUniversity of BirminghamEdgbastonUK
  4. 4.Department of PsychologyColumbia UniversityNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations